Definition
Imitation is the act of copying the behavior of someone observed. It is the most common learning rule and, as behavior, can be observed among animals (Galef and Laland 2005) as well as among humans (Apesteguia et al. 2007; Horner and Whiten 2005). Not to imitate means to experiment or innovate, to try something new instead of choosing one of the behaviors that has been observed.
To learn means to acquire knowledge or understanding. Social learning is about learning within a social group as opposed to individual learning where only the learning of a single person is considered. Concern is for how individuals within the group learn, and hence how the whole group learns. Social learning does not require that individuals within the group actively change their understanding about the environment. To exhibit learning as a group, it is already sufficient that more within the group adapt a superior action.
Theoretical Background
Im...
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Buying options
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Learn about institutional subscriptionsReferences
Alos-Ferrer, C., & Schlag, K. H. (2009). Imitation and social learning. In P. Anand, P. Pattanaik, & C. Puppe (Eds.), The handbook of rational and social choice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Apesteguia, J., Huck, S., & Oechssler, J. (2007). Imitation – theory and experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Theory, 136, 217–235.
Bjornerstedt, J., & Schlag, K. H. (1996). On the evolution of imitative behavior. Discussion Paper No. B.378, Sonderforschungsbereich 303, University of Bonn.
Galef, B. G., Jr., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. Bioscience, 55(6), 489–499.
Hofbauer, J., & Schlag, K. H. (2000). Sophisticated imitation in cyclic games. Journal of Evolutionary Economics, 10(5), 523–543.
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.
Kendal, J. R., Rendell, L., Pike, T. W., & Laland, K. N. (2009). Nine-spined sticklebacks deploy a hill-climbing social learning strategy. Behavioral Ecology, 20(2), 238–244.
Pike, T. W., Kendal, J. R., Rendell, L. E., & Laland, K. N. (2010). Learning by proportional observation in a species of fish. Behavioral Ecology, 21(3), 570–575.
Schlag, K. H. (1996). Imitate best vs. Imitate best average. Mimeo, University of Bonn. http://homepage.univie.ac.at/karl.schlag/research/imitation/IDIO9.pdf.
Schlag, K. H. (1998). Why imitate, and if so, how? A boundedly rational approach to multi-armed bandits. Journal of Economic Theory, 78(1), 130–156.
Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99–118.
Weibull, J. (1995). Evolutionary game theory. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC
About this entry
Cite this entry
Schlag, K.H. (2012). Imitation and Social Learning. In: Seel, N.M. (eds) Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_1452
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_1452
Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA
Print ISBN: 978-1-4419-1427-9
Online ISBN: 978-1-4419-1428-6
eBook Packages: Humanities, Social Sciences and LawReference Module Humanities and Social Sciences