Skip to main content
  • 123 Accesses

Definition

An innate, unlearned reflex that is elicited in response (unconditioned response) to a biologically-significant stimulus (unconditioned stimulus) will typically show habituation if the stimulus is repeated. That is, measures of the response will show orderly decreases in magnitude as the stimulus is repeatedly applied. This change in behavior, known as habituation, can be seen in the withdrawal responses of various invertebrates, including protozoa to tactile stimulation, in freezing-defensive responses of rodents to auditory stimuli, and in the orienting responses of human infants to complex auditory and or visual stimuli, and is one of the most fundamental properties of behavior. More simply put, habituation is the process by which a stimulus loses its attention-grabbing properties, i.e., its “novelty,” and decreases in responsiveness to the stimulus are seen when the stimulus is presented repeatedly or for an extended time period. An organism ceases to engage in attending...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 949.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 549.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  1. Balkenius, C. (2000). Attention, habituation and conditioning: Toward a computational model. Retrieved November 11, 2008, from http://www.lucs.lu.se/Christian.Balkenius/PDF/Balkenius.2000.CSQ.pdf

  2. Benaisch, A. A., & Leevers, H. J. (2003). Processing of rapidly presented auditory cues. in infancy: Implications for later language development. In H. Hayne & J. Fagen (Eds.), Progress in infancy research (Vol. 3, pp. 245–288). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bouton, M. E. (2007). Learning and behavior: A contemporary synthesis. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Brian, J. A., Landry, R., Szatmari, P., Niccols, A., & Byson, S. (2003). Habituation in high-risk infants: Reliability and patterns of responding. Infant and Child Development, 12, 387–394.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Eisenstein, E. M., Eisenstein, D., & Smith, J. C. (2001). The evolutionary significance of habituation and sensitization across phylogeny: A behavioral homeostasis model. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 36(4), 251–265.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Fagen, J. W., & Ohr, P. S. (2001). Learning and memory in infancy: Habituation, instrumental conditioning, and expectancy formation. In L. T. Singer & P. S. Zeskind (Eds.), Biobehavioral assessment of the infant (pp. 233–273). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Gluck, M. A., Mercado, E., & Myers, C. E. (2008). Learning and memory: From brain to behavior. New York: Worth.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Groves, P. M., & Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: A dual process theory. Psychological Review, 77, 419–450.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hoben, T., & Gilmore, R. O. (2004). Habituation assessment in infancy. Psychological Methods, 9(1), 70–92.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (2000). Cellular mechanisms of learning and the biological basis of individuality. In E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, & T. M. Jessell (Eds.), Principles of neural science (4th ed., pp. 1247–1279). New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  11. McSweeney, F. K., & Swindell, S. (2002). Common processes may contribute to extinction and habituation. Journal of General Psychology, 129(4), 1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  12. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2001). From cells to selves: Biobehavioral development. Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Pear, J. J. (2001). The science of learning. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2001). Attention and recognition memory in the 1st year of life: A longitudinal study of preterm and full-term infants. Developmental Psychology, 37, 539–549.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Rose, S. A., & Orlian, E. K. (2001). Visual information processing. In L. T. Singer & P. S. Zeskind (Eds.), Biobehavioral assessment of the infant (pp. 274–292). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Rovee-Collier, C., & Barr, R. (2002). Infant cognition. In H. Pashler & J. Wixted (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 693–791). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Rudy, J. W. (2008). The neurobiology of learning and memory. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Terry, W. S. (2009). Learning and memory: Basic principles, processes and procedures. Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 73, 16–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this entry

Cite this entry

Phelps, B.I. (2011). Habituation. In: Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J.A. (eds) Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_1302

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_1302

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-387-77579-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-387-79061-9

  • eBook Packages: Behavioral Science

Publish with us

Policies and ethics