Encyclopedia of Complexity and Systems Science

2009 Edition
| Editors: Robert A. Meyers (Editor-in-Chief)

Development, Complex Dynamic Systems of

  • Paul van Geert
Reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-30440-3_120

Definition of the Subject

Developmental psychology concerns the study of developmental changes in human beings across the life span. Developmental changes are broadly defined as changes in the organism that are mostly progressive – in terms of increasing complexity, adaptation to the environment, efficiency of actions and operations and so forth. The subject of developmental psychology is the individual person, embedded in a particular social, cultural and material context. Although it is now widely accepted that development is a life-long process, the main developmental events take place during the first part of the life span, which co-occur with physical growth and development and coincide roughly with the age from birth to adulthood. Developmental changes through adulthood and old-age are often referred to as processes of aging and imply processes of loss, for instancing due to aging of the nervous system, and processes compensating for such losses.

Developmental psychology is...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Bibliography

Primary Literature

  1. 1.
    Abramovitch R, Grusec JE (1978) Peer imitation in a natural setting. Child Dev 49:60–65Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Allen P, Strathern M (2003) Evolution, emergence, and learning in complex systems. Emerg 5(4):8–33Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Anderson JR (1996) ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. Am Psychol 51:355–365Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Anderson JR, Bothell D, Byrne MD, Douglass S, Lebiere C, Qin Y (2004) An integrated theory of the mind. Psychol Rev 111(4):1036–1060Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Asbury K, Wachs T, Plomin R (2005) Environmental moderators of genetic influence on verbal and nonverbal abilities in early childhood. Intell 33(6):643–661Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Atkinson M (1986) Learning and models of development. In: van Geert P (ed) Theory building in developmental psychology. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 105–139)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Axelrod R (1997) The complexity of cooperation: agent-based models of competition and collaboration. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Bailey DB, Bruer JT, Symons FJ, Lichtman JW (2001) Critical thinking about critical periods. Brookes Publishing, BaltimoreGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Bak P, Sneppen K (1993) Punctuated equilibrium and criticality in a simple model of evolution. Phys Rev Lett 71(24):4083–4086Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Bak P (1996) How Nature works: the science of self‐organized criticality. Springer, New YorkzbMATHGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Ball P (2005) Critical mass: how one thing leads to another. Arrow Books/Random House, LondonGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Ballato L, van Geert P, Schumacher N (2007) A dynamic systems model of friendship interaction and risk behavior in adolescence: short‐and‐long term aspects of influence and selection mechanisms. Internal paper. Groningen: Department of Developmental PsychologyGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Baltes PB, Reese H, Lipsitt L (1980) Lifespan developmental psychology. Ann Rev Pyschol 31:65–110Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Bandura A, Bussey K (2004) On broadening the cognitive, motivational, and sociostructural scope of theorizing about gender development and functioning: comment on Martin, Ruble, and Szkrybalo (2002). Psycholog Bull 130:691–701Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Bandura A, Walters R (1977) Social learning and personality development. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, LondonGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Bandura A, Ross D, Ross SA (1963) A comparative test of the status envy, social power, and secondary reinforcement theories of identificatory learning. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 67:527–534Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Bankes S, Lempert R (2004) Robust reasoning with agent-based modeling. Nonlin Dyn Psychol Life Sci 8(2):259–277Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Bankes S, Lempert R, Popper S (2002) Making computational social science effective: epistemology, methodology, and technology. Soc Sci Comput Rev 20(4):377–388Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Barsalou L, Breazeal C, Smith L (2007) Cognition as coordinated non‐cognition. Cogn Process 8(2):79–91Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Bassano D, van Geert P (2007) Modeling continuity and discontinuity in utterance length: a quantitative approach to changes, transitions and intra‐individual variability in early grammatical development. Dev Sci 10(5):588–612Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Baron‐Cohen S, Tager‐Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (1993) The impairment of ToMM: some issues. In: Baron‐Cohen S, Tager‐Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (eds) Understanding other minds. Perspectives form autism. Oxford University Press, pp 102–105Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Battro AM (2001) Half a brain is enough: the story of Nico. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Baum WM, Davison M (2004) Choice in a variable environment: visit patterns in the dynamics of choice. J Exp Anal Behav 81:85–127Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Belpaeme T, Cowley S (2007) Foreword: extending symbol grounding. Interact Stud 8(1):1–6Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Berger KS (2003) The developing person through childhood and adolescence, 6th edn. Worth Publishers, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Berthouze L, Metta G (2005) Epigenetic robotics: modelling cognitive development in robotic systems, Cogn Syst Res 6(3):189–192Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Berthouze L, Ziemke T (2003) Epigenetic robotics‐modelling cognitive development in robotic systems. Connect Sci 15(4)147:150Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Berthouze L, Metta G, Sun R (2005) Editorial: Epigenetic robotics: Modelling cognitive development in robotic systems. Cogn Syst Res 6(3):189–192Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Bianchi‐Berthouze N, Kleinsmith A (2003) A categorical approach to affective gesture recognition. Connect Sci 15(4):259–269Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Billington E, DiTommaso N (2003) Demonstrations and applications of the matching law in education. J Behav Educ 12(2):91–104Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Biro S, Leslie A (2007) Infants' perception of goal‐directed actions: development through cue-based bootstrapping. Dev Sci 10(3):379–398Google Scholar
  32. 32.
    Bjorklund D, Pellegrini A (2000) Child development and evolutionary psychology. Child Dev 71(6):1687–1708Google Scholar
  33. 33.
    Bjorklund D, Smith P (2003) Evolutionary developmental psychology: Introduction to the special issue. J Exp Child Psychol 85(3):195–198Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Boccia M, Pedersen C (2001) Animal models of critical and sensitive periods in social and emotional development. In: Critical thinking about critical periods. Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, pp 107–127Google Scholar
  35. 35.
    Boettcher S, Paczuski M (1996) Exact Results for Spatiotemporal Correlations in a Self‐Organized Critical Model of Punctuated Equilibrium. Phys Rev Lett 76(3):348–351ADSGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Boettcher S, Percus A (2000) Nature's way of optimizing. Artif Intell 119:275–286zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Bongers R, Smitsman A, Michaels C (2003) Geometrics and dynamics of a rod determine how it is used for reaching. J Motor Behav 35(1):4–22Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Borrero J, Vollmer T (2002) An application of the matching law to severe problem behavior. J Appl Behav Anal 35(1):13–27Google Scholar
  39. 39.
    Borenstein E, Ruppin E (2005) The evolution of imitation and mirror neurons in adaptive agents, Cogn Syst Res 6(3):229–242Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Bortfeld H, Whitehurst G (2001) Sensitive periods in first language acquisition. In: Critical thinking about critical periods. Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, pp 173–192Google Scholar
  41. 41.
    Bosma H, Kunnen E (2001) Determinants and mechanisms in ego identity development: a review and synthesis. Dev Rev 21(1):39–66Google Scholar
  42. 42.
    Bosma H, Kunnen E (2001) Identity and emotion: Development through self‐organization. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Boyce WT, Ellis BJ (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary‐developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol 17:271–301Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Breazeal C (2003) Emotion and sociable humanoid robots. Int J Human–Comput Stud 59(1):119–155Google Scholar
  45. 45.
    Breazeal C, Brooks R (2005) Robot emotion: a functional perspective. In: Who needs emotions: the brain meets the robot. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 271–310Google Scholar
  46. 46.
    Breazeal C, Scassellati B (2000) Infant‐like social interactions between a robot and a human caregiver. Adapt Behav 8(1):49–74Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Breazeal C, Scassellati B (2002) Robots that imitate humans. Trends Cogn Sci 6(11):481–487Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Brighton H (2002) Compositional syntax from cultural transmission. Artif Life 8:25–54Google Scholar
  49. 49.
    Bruer J (2001) A critical and sensitive period primer. In: Critical thinking about critical periods. Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, pp 3–26Google Scholar
  50. 50.
    Bruer J (2002) Avoiding the pediatrician's error: how neuroscientists can help educators (and themselves). Nature Neurosci 5:1031–1033Google Scholar
  51. 51.
    Bukatko D, Daehler MW (2001) Child development: a thematic approach, 4th edn. Houghton, Mifflin and Company, BostonGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    Bussey K, Bandura A (1984) Influence of gender constancy and social power on sex‐linked modeling. J Personal Soc Psychol 47:1292–1302Google Scholar
  53. 53.
    Call J, Carpenter M (2002) Three sources of information in social learning. In: Dautenhahn K, Nehaniv CL (eds) Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 211–2228Google Scholar
  54. 54.
    Camras L, Witherington D (2005) Dynamical systems approaches to emotional development. Dev Rev 25(3):328–350Google Scholar
  55. 55.
    Camras L, Oster H, Bakeman R, Meng Z, Ujiie T, Campos J (2007) Do infants show distinct negative facial expressions for fear and anger? Emotional expression in 11-month-old European American, Chinese, and Japanese infants. Infancy 11(2):131–155Google Scholar
  56. 56.
    Cañamero L, Gaussier P (2005) Emotion understanding: robots as tools and models. In: Emotional development: recent research advances. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 235–258Google Scholar
  57. 57.
    Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2005) Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. Dev Sci 8:13–20Google Scholar
  58. 58.
    Case R (1992) The mind's staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children's thought and knowledge. Lawrence Earlbaum, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  59. 59.
    Cashon CH, Cohen LB (2004) Beyond U-shaped development in infants' processing of faces: an information‐processing account. J Cogn Dev 5:59–80Google Scholar
  60. 60.
    Casti JL (1994) Complexification: explaining a paradoxical world through the science of surprise. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Chomsky N (1957) Syntactic structures. The Hague, MoutonGoogle Scholar
  62. 62.
    Chomsky N (1959) A review of Bf Skinner's verbal behavior. Lang 35(1):26–58Google Scholar
  63. 63.
    Chomsky N (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  64. 64.
    Chomsky N (1995) The minimalist program. MIT Press, Cambridge MAzbMATHGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Christiansen MH, Dale R (2003) Language evolution and change. In: Arbib MA (ed) Handbook of brain theory and neural networks, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 604–606Google Scholar
  66. 66.
    Christiansen MH, Kirby S (2003) Language evolution: consensus and controversies. Trends Cogn Sci 7(7):300–307Google Scholar
  67. 67.
    Cilliers P (2002) Why we cannot know complex things completely. Emerg 4(1/2):77–84Google Scholar
  68. 68.
    Clark A (1997) Being there: putting brain, body and world together again. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  69. 69.
    Clark A (2006) Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends Cogn Sci 10:370–374Google Scholar
  70. 70.
    Clark A (2006) Material symbols. Philos Psychol 19(3):291–307Google Scholar
  71. 71.
    Clark A, Chalmers D (1998) The extended mind. Anal 58:7–19Google Scholar
  72. 72.
    Clowes R (2007) Semiotic symbols and the missing theory of thinking. Interact Stud 8(1):105–124Google Scholar
  73. 73.
    Cole M Cole SR (1993) The development of children, 2nd edn. Scientific American Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  74. 74.
    Colunga E, Smith L (2005, April) From the Lexicon to Expectations About Kinds: A Role for Associative Learning. Psychol Rev 112(2):347–382Google Scholar
  75. 75.
    Cowley S (2007) How human infants deal with symbol grounding. Interact Stud 8(1):83–104Google Scholar
  76. 76.
    Cruchtfield JP (1994) The calculi of emergence: computation, dynamics and induction. Physica D 75:11–54ADSGoogle Scholar
  77. 77.
    Cziko G (2000) The things we do using the lessons of Bernard and Darwin to understand the what, how, and why of our behavior. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  78. 78.
    Dahl R, Spear L (2004) Adolescent brain development: Vulnerabilities and opportunities. New York Academy of Sciences, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  79. 79.
    Davidson M, Amso D, Anderson L, Diamond A (2006) Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychol 44(11):2037–2078Google Scholar
  80. 80.
    Dawson‐Tunik T, Commons M, Wilson M, Fischer K (2005, June) The shape of development. Eur J Dev Psychol 2(2):163–195Google Scholar
  81. 81.
    de Hamilton CA, Brindley R, Frith U (2007) Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia 45(8):1859–1868Google Scholar
  82. 82.
    Demetriou A, Kyriakides L (2006, June) The functional and developmental organization of cognitive developmental sequences. Br J Educational Psychol 76(2):209–242Google Scholar
  83. 83.
    Demiris Y, Johnson M (2003) Distributed, predictive perception of actions: a biologically inspired robotics architecture for imitation and learning. Connect Sci 15(4):231–243Google Scholar
  84. 84.
    Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S (2007) The early years: preschool program improves cognitive control, Sci 318:1387–1388Google Scholar
  85. 85.
    Dishion T, Bullock B, Granic I (2002, September) Pragmatism in modeling peer influence: Dynamics, outcomes and change processes. Dev Psychopathol 14(4):969–981Google Scholar
  86. 86.
    Dominey PF (2007) Towards a construction‐based framework for development of language, event perception and social cognition: insights from grounded robotics and simulation. Neurocomput 70(13):2288–2302Google Scholar
  87. 87.
    Dominey PF, Boucher J-D (2005) Developmental stages of perception and language acquisition in a perceptually grounded robot, Cogn Syst Res 6(3):243–259Google Scholar
  88. 88.
    Dooley K (2002) Organizational complexity In: Warner M (ed) International encyclopedia of business and management. Thompson Learning, London, pp 5013–5022Google Scholar
  89. 89.
    Dooley K, Corman S (2002) Agent-based, genetic, and emergent computational models of complex systems. In: Kiel LD (ed) Encyclopedia of life support systems (EOLSS). UNESCO/EOLSS, Oxford UK, http://www.eolss.net
  90. 90.
    Duncan G, Dowsett C, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston A, Klebanov P et al (2007) School readiness and later achievement. Dev Psychol 43(6):1428–1446Google Scholar
  91. 91.
    Eggleston R, McCreight K, Young M (2005) Distributed cognition and situated behavior. Modeling human behavior with integrated cognitive architectures: comparison, evaluation, and validation. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 177–235Google Scholar
  92. 92.
    Elbers E (2004) Conversational asymmetry and the child's perspective in developmental and educational research. Int J Disabil Dev Educ 51(2):201–215Google Scholar
  93. 93.
    Eley T, Sugden K, Corsico A, Gregory A, Sham P, McGuffin P et al (2004) Gene‐environment interaction analysis of serotonin system markers with adolescent depression. Mol Psychiatry 9(10):908–915Google Scholar
  94. 94.
    Elliott E, Kiel L (2004) Agent-based modeling in the social and behavioral sciences. Nonlin Dyn Psychol Life Sci 8(2):121–130Google Scholar
  95. 95.
    Ellis BJ (2004) Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychol Bull 130:920–958Google Scholar
  96. 96.
    Ellis B, Bjorklund D (2005) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  97. 97.
    Ellis BJ, Essex MJ, Boyce WT (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary‐developmental theory. Dev Psychopathol 17:303–328Google Scholar
  98. 98.
    Eoyang G (2004) The practitioner's landscape. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(1/2):55–60Google Scholar
  99. 99.
    Erlhagen W, Schöner G (2002, July) Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychol Rev 109(3):545–572Google Scholar
  100. 100.
    Erikson EH (1968) Identity: youth and crisis. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  101. 101.
    Fioretti G, Visser B (2004) A cognitive interpretation of organizational complexity. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(1/2):11–23Google Scholar
  102. 102.
    Fischer KW (1980) A theory of cognitive development: the control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychol Rev 87:477–531Google Scholar
  103. 103.
    Fischer KW, Bidell TR (2006) Dynamic development of action, thought, and emotion. In: Damon W, Lerner RM (eds) Theoretical models of human development. Handbook of child psychology, vol 1, 6th edn. Wiley, New York, pp 313–399Google Scholar
  104. 104.
    Fischer KW, Kennedy B (1997) Tools for analyzing the many shapes of development: the case of self-in‐relationships in Korea. In: Change and development: issues of theory, method, and application. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 117–152Google Scholar
  105. 105.
    Fischer KW, Rose SP (1999) Rulers, models, and nonlinear dynamics: measurement and method in developmental research. In: Savelsbergh G, van der Maas H, van Geert P (eds) Nonlinear developmental processes, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, pp 197–212Google Scholar
  106. 106.
    Fitzpatrick P, Arsenio A, Torres-Jara E (2006) Reinforcing robot perception of multi-modal events through repetition and redundancy and repetition and redundancy. Interact Stud 7(2):171–196Google Scholar
  107. 107.
    Fogel A (1993) Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  108. 108.
    Ford DH Lerner RM (1992) Developmental systems theory: an integrative approach. Sage Publications, Thousand OaksGoogle Scholar
  109. 109.
    Förster J, Liberman N, Friedman R (2007) Seven principles of goal activation: a systematic approach to distinguishing goal priming from priming of non-goal constructs. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 11(3):211–233Google Scholar
  110. 110.
    Frith U, Frith CD (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philos Trans Royal Soc Lond Biol Sci 358:459–473Google Scholar
  111. 111.
    Frith U, Hill E (2003) Autism: Mind and brain. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  112. 112.
    Frith U, Morton J, Leslie A (1991) The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism. Trends Neurosci 14(10):433–438Google Scholar
  113. 113.
    Gallese V (2006) Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Res 1079:15–24Google Scholar
  114. 114.
    Gallistel CR, Gibbon J (2000) Time, rate, and conditioning. Psychol Rev 107:289–344Google Scholar
  115. 115.
    Geary D (2006) Evolutionary developmental psychology: current status and future directions. Dev Rev 26(2):113–119Google Scholar
  116. 116.
    Gergely G (2003) What should a robot learn from an infant? Mechanisms of action interpretation and observational learning in infancy. Connect Sci 15(4):191–209Google Scholar
  117. 117.
    Gershkoff-Stowe L, Thelen E (2004) U-shaped changes in behavior: a dynamic systems perspective. J Cogn Dev 5:11–36Google Scholar
  118. 118.
    Gibson J (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  119. 119.
    Goldin‐Meadow S, Wagner S (2005) How our hands help us learn. Trends Cogn Sci 9(5):234–241Google Scholar
  120. 120.
    Goldspink C (2007) Transforming education: evidential support for a complex systems approach. Emerg: Complex Organ 9(1/2):77–92Google Scholar
  121. 121.
    Goldstein J (1999) Emergence as a construct: History and issues. Emergence 1:49–72Google Scholar
  122. 122.
    Goldstein J (2002, October) The Singular Nature of Emergent Levels: Suggestions for a Theory of Emergence. Nonlinear Dyn Psychol Life Sci 6(4):293–309Google Scholar
  123. 123.
    Goldstone R, Janssen M (2005) Computational models of collective behavior. Trends Cogn Sci 9(9):424–430Google Scholar
  124. 124.
    Gottfried AW, Gottfried AE (1974) Influence of social power vs status envy modeled behaviors on children's preferences for models. Psycholog Rep 34:1147–1150Google Scholar
  125. 125.
    Gottlieb G (2001) A developmental psychobiological systems view: early formulation and current status. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  126. 126.
    Gottman JM, Murray JD, Swanson CC, Tyson R, Swanson KR (2002) The mathematics of marriage: dynamic nonlinear models. MIT Press, CambridgezbMATHGoogle Scholar
  127. 127.
    Granic I, Hollenstein T (2003) Dynamic systems methods for models of developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 15(3):641–669Google Scholar
  128. 128.
    Granic I, Lamey A (2002) Combining dynamic systems and multivariate analyses to compare the mother‐child interactions of externalizing subtypes. J Abnorm Child Psychol 30(3):265–283Google Scholar
  129. 129.
    Granic I, Hollenstein T, Dishion T, Patterson G (2003) Longitudinal analysis of flexibility and reorganization in early adolescence: a dynamic systems study of family interactions. Dev Psychol 39(3):606–617Google Scholar
  130. 130.
    Granott N (2002) How microdevelopment creates macrodevelopment: Reiterated sequences, backward transitions, and the Zone of Current Development. Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 213–242Google Scholar
  131. 131.
    Grusec JE (1992) Social learning theory and developmental psychology: the legacies of Robert Sears and Albert Bandura. Dev Psychol 28:776–786Google Scholar
  132. 132.
    Grusec JE, Abramovitch R (1982) Imitation of peers and adults in a natural setting: a functional analysis. Child Dev 53:636–642Google Scholar
  133. 133.
    Guerin S, Kunkle D (2004) Emergence of constraint in self‐organizing systems. Nonlin Dyn Psychol Life Sci 8(2):131–146Google Scholar
  134. 134.
    Gulyás L (2002) On the transition to agent-based modeling: Implementation strategies from variables to agents. Soc Sci Comput Rev 20(4):389–399Google Scholar
  135. 135.
    Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Tager‐Flusberg H (2006) Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism. Cereb Cortex 16:1276–1282Google Scholar
  136. 136.
    Hamaker EL, Dolan CV, Molenaar PCM (2005) Statistical modeling of the individual: rationale and application of multivariate stationary time series analysis. Multivar Behav Res 40:207–233Google Scholar
  137. 137.
    Hammond S, Sanders M (2002) Dialogue as social self‐organization: an introduction. Emerg 4(4):7–24Google Scholar
  138. 138.
    Harré R (2000) Acts of living. Sci 289(25):1303–1304Google Scholar
  139. 139.
    Hartelman PA, van der Maas HLJ, Molenaar PCM (1998) Detecting and modelling developmental transitions. Br J Dev Psychol 16:97–122Google Scholar
  140. 140.
    Henrickson L (2002) Old wine in a new wineskin: college choice, college access using agent-based modeling. Soc Sci Comput Rev 20(4):400–419Google Scholar
  141. 141.
    Hernández Blasi C, Bjorklund D (2003) Evolutionary developmental psychology: a new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Dev 46(5):259–281Google Scholar
  142. 142.
    Herrnstein RJ (1961) Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 4:267–272Google Scholar
  143. 143.
    Herrnstein RJ (1970) On the law of effect. J Exp Anal Behav 13:243–266Google Scholar
  144. 144.
    Herrnstein RJ, Prelec D (1991) Melioration: A theory of distributed choice. J Econom Perspect 5:137–156Google Scholar
  145. 145.
    Hodgson G (2000) The concept of emergence in social sciences: its history and importance. Emerg 2(4):65–77Google Scholar
  146. 146.
    Holland JH (1995) Hidden order: How adaptation builds complexity. Addison‐Wesley, ReadingGoogle Scholar
  147. 147.
    Holland JH (1998) Emergence. From chaos to order. Addison‐Wesley, Redwood CityzbMATHGoogle Scholar
  148. 148.
    Hollenstein T (2007) State space grids: analyzing dynamics across development. Int J Beh Dev 31(4):384–396Google Scholar
  149. 149.
    Hollenstein T, Lewis M (2006) A state space analysis of emotion and flexibility in parent‐child interactions. Emot 6(4):656–662Google Scholar
  150. 150.
    Hosenfeld B, van der Maas HLJ, van den Boom DC (1997a) Detecting bimodality in the analogical reasoning performance of elementary schoolchildren. Int J Behav Dev 20:529–547Google Scholar
  151. 151.
    Hosenfeld B, van der Maas HLJ, van den Boom DC (1997b) Indicators of discontinuous change in the development of analogical reasoning. J Exp Child Psychol 64:367–395Google Scholar
  152. 152.
    Howe ML, Lewis MD (2005) The importance of dynamic systems approaches for understanding development. Dev Rev 25:247–251Google Scholar
  153. 153.
    Iacoboni M, Dapretto M (2006) The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:942–951Google Scholar
  154. 154.
    Immordino-Yang MH (2005) A tale of two cases: emotion and affective prosody after hemispherectomy. ProQuest Information LearningGoogle Scholar
  155. 155.
    Immordino-Yang MH (2007) A tale of two cases. Lessons for education from the study of two boys living with half their brains. Mind Brain Educ 1(2):66–83Google Scholar
  156. 156.
    Iverson J, Goldin‐Meadow S (2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychol Sci 16(5):367–371Google Scholar
  157. 157.
    Jansen BRJ, van der Maas HLJ (2001b) Evidence for the Phase transition from rule I to rule II on the balance scale task. Developmental Review, 21, 450–494Google Scholar
  158. 158.
    Jansen BRJ, van der Maas HLJ (2002b) The development of children's rule use on the balance scale task. J Exp Child Psychol 81:383–416Google Scholar
  159. 159.
    Johnson M (2005) Sensitive periods in functional brain development: problems and prospects. Dev Psychobiol 46(3):287–292Google Scholar
  160. 160.
    Jones G (2007) Agent-based modeling: Use with necessary caution. Am J Public Health 97(5):780–781Google Scholar
  161. 161.
    Jones G, Ritter FE, Wood DJ (2000) Using a cognitive architecture to examine what develops. Psychol Sci 11(2):1–8Google Scholar
  162. 162.
    Kail RV (2001) Children and their development, 2nd edn. Prentice-Hall, Englewood CliffsGoogle Scholar
  163. 163.
    Kaplan F, Hafner V (2006) The challenges of joint attention. Interact Stud 7(2):135–169Google Scholar
  164. 164.
    Ke J, Holland J (2006) Language origin from an emergentist perspective. Appl Linguist 27(4):691–716Google Scholar
  165. 165.
    Kello CT (2004) Characterizing the evolutionary dynamics of language. Trends Cogn Sci 8:392–394Google Scholar
  166. 166.
    Kendal J (2006) Review of: Galef BG Jr, Heyes CM (2004) Social learning and imitation. Learn Behav 32(1):1–140. Interact Stud 7(2):273–288ADSGoogle Scholar
  167. 167.
    Kerpelman J, Pittman J, Lamke L (1997) Toward a microprocess perspective on adolescent identity development: an identity control theory approach. J Adolesc Res 12(3):325–346Google Scholar
  168. 168.
    Kirby S (2002) Natural language from artificial life. Artif Life 8:185–215Google Scholar
  169. 169.
    Kirby S, Smith K, Brighton H (2004) From UG to universals: linguistic adaptation through iterated learning. Stud Lang 28:587–607Google Scholar
  170. 170.
    Kitchener K, Lynch C, Fischer K, Wood P (1993) Developmental range of reflective judgment: the effect of contextual support and practice on developmental stage. Dev Psychol 29(5):893–906Google Scholar
  171. 171.
    Klahr D, Wallace JG (1976) Cognitive development: an information processing approach. Lawrence Erlbaum, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  172. 172.
    Knafo A, Plomin R (2006) Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Dev Psychol 42(5):771–786Google Scholar
  173. 173.
    Knudsen E (2004) Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J Cogn Neurosci 16(8):1412–1425Google Scholar
  174. 174.
    Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E (2007) Children's and adults' neural bases of verbal and nonverbal ‘theory of mind’. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1522–1532Google Scholar
  175. 175.
    Kolb B, Whishaw IQ (1998) Brain plasticity and behavior. Ann Rev Psychol 49:43–64Google Scholar
  176. 176.
    Kouritzin S (1999) Face[t]s of first language loss. Lawrence Erlbaum, MahwahGoogle Scholar
  177. 177.
    Kovas Y, Plomin R (2007) Learning abilities and disabilities: generalist genes, specialist environments. Current Dir Psychol Sci 16(5):284–288Google Scholar
  178. 178.
    Kovas Y, Petrill S, Plomin R (2007) The origins of diverse domains of mathematics: generalist genes but specialist environments. J Educ Psychol 99(1):128–139Google Scholar
  179. 179.
    Kroger J (2004) Identity in adolescence. The balance between self and other, 3rd edn. Routledge, East SussexGoogle Scholar
  180. 180.
    Kunnen E, Bosma H (2000) Development of meaning making: a dynamic systems approach. New Ideas Psychol 18(1):57–82Google Scholar
  181. 181.
    Lasnik H (2002) The minimalist program in syntax. Trends Cogn Sci 6(10):432–437Google Scholar
  182. 182.
    Lee K, Park N, Song H (2005) Can a robot be perceived as a developing creature? Effects of a robot's long-term cognitive developments on its social presence and people's social responses toward it. Human Commun Res 31(4):538–563MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  183. 183.
    Lepage JF, Théoret H (2006) EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation‐execution matching system in children. Eur J Neurosci 23:2505–2510Google Scholar
  184. 184.
    Lerner RM (2006) Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary theories of human development. Wiley, HobokenGoogle Scholar
  185. 185.
    Lewis M (1995, March) Cognition‐emotion feedback and the self‐organization of developmental paths. Hum Dev 38(2):71–102Google Scholar
  186. 186.
    Lewis M (2000, January) The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account of human development. Child Dev 71(1):36–43Google Scholar
  187. 187.
    Lewis MD (2005) Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behav Brain Sci 28:169–245Google Scholar
  188. 188.
    Lewis MD (2005) Self‐organizing individual differences in brain development. Dev Rev 25(3):252–277Google Scholar
  189. 189.
    Lewis MD, Todd R (2007) The self‐regulating brain: cortical‐subcortical feedback and the development of intelligent action. Cogn Dev 22(4):406–430Google Scholar
  190. 190.
    Lewis MD, Lamey A, Douglas L (1999) A new dynamic systems method for the analysis of early socioemotional development. Dev Sci 2(4):457–475Google Scholar
  191. 191.
    Lewis MD, Zimmerman S, Hollenstein T, Lamey A (2004) Reorganization in coping behavior at 1 years: Dynamic systems and normative change. Dev Sci 7(1):56–73Google Scholar
  192. 192.
    Lewis T, Maurer D (2005) Multiple sensitive periods in human visual development: evidence from visually deprived children. Dev Psychobiol 46(3):163–183Google Scholar
  193. 193.
    Lichtwarck‐Aschoff A, van Geert P, Bosma H, Kunnen S (2008) Time and identity: a framework for research and theory formation. Dev Rev 28(3):370–400Google Scholar
  194. 194.
    Lickliter R, Honeycutt H (2003) Developmental dynamics: toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psycholog Bull 129:819–835Google Scholar
  195. 195.
    Lickliter R, Honeycutt H (2003) Evolutionary approaches to cognitive development: status and strategy. J Cogn Dev 4(4):459–473Google Scholar
  196. 196.
    Lieberman MD (2007) Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Ann Rev Psychol 58:259–289Google Scholar
  197. 197.
    Lindblom J, Ziemke T (2006) The social body in motion: cognitive development in infants and androids. Connect Sci 18(4):333–346Google Scholar
  198. 198.
    Locke J (2007) Bimodal signaling in infancy: Motor behavior, reference, and the evolution of spoken language. Interact Stud 8(1):159–175MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  199. 199.
    Lockman JJ (2000) A perception–action perspective on tool use development. Child Dev 71(1):137–144Google Scholar
  200. 200.
    Lopes L, Chauhan A (2007) How many words can my robot learn? An approach and experiments with one-class learning. Interact Stud 8(1):53–81Google Scholar
  201. 201.
    Louro M, Pieters R, Zeelenberg M (2007) Dynamics of multiple‐goal pursuit. J Personal Soc Psychol 93(2):174–193Google Scholar
  202. 202.
    Lungarella M, Metta G, Pfeifer R, Sandini G (2003) Developmental robotics: a survey. Connect Sci 15(4):151–190Google Scholar
  203. 203.
    MacDorman K (2007) Afterword: life after the symbol system metaphor. Interact Stud 8(1):1–999Google Scholar
  204. 204.
    MacDorman K, Ishiguro H (2006) Toward social mechanisms of android science: a CogSci Workshop, 25 and 26 July 2005, Stresa, Italy. Interact Stud 7(2):289–296Google Scholar
  205. 205.
    Maestripieri D, Roney J (2006,). Evolutionary developmental psychology: contributions from comparative research with nonhuman primates. Dev Rev 26(2):120–137Google Scholar
  206. 206.
    Magnusson D (1999) On the individual: a person‐oriented approach to developmental research. Eur Psychol 4(4):205–218Google Scholar
  207. 207.
    Magnusson D (2001) The holistic‐interactionistic paradigm: some directions for empirical developmental research. Eur Psychol 6(3):153–162Google Scholar
  208. 208.
    Martin LC, Fabes RA, Hanish LD, Hollenstein T (2005) Social dynamics in the preschool. Dev Rev 25:299–327Google Scholar
  209. 209.
    Masten A (2007) Resilience in developing systems: progress and promise as the fourth wave rises. Dev Psychopath 19(3):921–930Google Scholar
  210. 210.
    McShane J (1991) Cognitive development: an information processing approach. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  211. 211.
    Metta G, Berthouze L (2006) Epigenetic robotics: modelling cognitive development in robotic systems. Interact Stud 7(2):129–134Google Scholar
  212. 212.
    Metta G, Sandini G, Natale L, Craighero L, Fadiga L (2006) Understanding mirror neurons: a bio‐robotic approach. Interact Stud 7(2):197–232Google Scholar
  213. 213.
    Mezic I, Gruenewald P, Gorman D, Mezic J (2007) Agent‐based modeling: use with necessary caution: reply. Am J Public Health 97(5):781–782Google Scholar
  214. 214.
    Mitchell P (1997) Introduction to theory of mind. Children, autism and apes. Arnold, LondenGoogle Scholar
  215. 215.
    Moldoveanu M (2004) An intersubjective measure of organizational complexity: a new approach to the study of complexity in organizations. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(3):9–26Google Scholar
  216. 216.
    Molenaar P (2004) A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Meas: Interdiscipl Res Perspect 2(4):201–218Google Scholar
  217. 217.
    Molenaar P, Huizenga H, Nesselroade J (2003) The relationship between the structure of interindividual and intraindividual variability: a theoretical and empirical vindication of developmental systems theory. In: Understanding human development: dialogues with lifespan psychology. Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  218. 218.
    Montague DPF, Walker‐Andrews AS (2001) Peekaboo: a new look at infants' perception of emotion expressions. Dev Psychol 37:826–838Google Scholar
  219. 219.
    Moore D (2003) Trying to fix the development in evolutionary developmental psychology. Am J Psychol 116(2):299–307Google Scholar
  220. 220.
    Mosekilde E (1998) Topics in nonlinear dynamics: applications to physics biology and economic systems. World Scientific, SingaporeGoogle Scholar
  221. 221.
    Moss S, Edmonds B (2005) Sociology and simulation: statistical and qualitative cross‐validation. Am J Sociol 110(4):1095–1131Google Scholar
  222. 222.
    Munakata Y, Casey BJ, Diamond A (2004) Developmental cognitive neuroscience: progress and potential. Trends Cogn Sci 8(3): 122–128Google Scholar
  223. 223.
    Murray L, Kollins S (2000) Effects of methylphenidate on sensitivity to reinforcement in children diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an application of the matching law. J Appl Behav Anal 33(4):573–591Google Scholar
  224. 224.
    Musher‐Eizenman DR, Nesselroade JR, Schmitz B (2002) Perceived control and academic performance: a comparison of high- and low‐performing children on within‐person change patterns. Int J Behav Dev 26:540–547Google Scholar
  225. 225.
    Nagai Y, Hosoda K, Morita A, Asada M (2003) A constructive model for the development of joint attention. Connect Sci 15(4):211–229Google Scholar
  226. 226.
    Namy LL, Campbell AL, Tomasello M (2004) The changing role of iconicity in non‐verbal symbol learning: a U‐shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures. J Cogn Dev 5:37–57Google Scholar
  227. 227.
    Newman BM Newman PR (2006) Development through life: a psychosocial approach. Thomson Wadsworth, BelmontGoogle Scholar
  228. 228.
    Niyogi P, Berwick RC (2001) A dynamical systems model for language change. Complex Syst 11(3):161–204MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  229. 229.
    Nowak MA, Komarova NL (2001) Towards an evolutionary theory of language. Trends Cogn Sci 5(7):288–295Google Scholar
  230. 230.
    Nowak MA, Komarova NL, Niyogi P (2001) Evolution of universal grammar. Sci 291:114–118MathSciNetzbMATHADSGoogle Scholar
  231. 231.
    Nowak MA, Komarova NL, Niyogi P (2002) Computational and evolutionary aspects of language. Nature 417:611—617Google Scholar
  232. 232.
    Oberman LM, Ramachandran VS (2007) The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Bull 133:310–327Google Scholar
  233. 233.
    Oyama S, Griffiths PE, Gray RD (2001) Cycles of contingency: developmental systems and evolution. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  234. 234.
    Pan B, Gleason J (1986) The study of language loss: models and hypotheses for an emerging discipline. Appl Psycholinguist 7(3):193–206Google Scholar
  235. 235.
    Pellegrini A, Bjorklund D (2004) The ontogeny and phylogeny of children's object and fantasy play. Human Nature 15(1):23–43Google Scholar
  236. 236.
    Pepper SC (1926) Emergence, The J Philos 23(9):241–245Google Scholar
  237. 237.
    Pepper SC (2004) Emergence. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(4):66–245Google Scholar
  238. 238.
    Pessa E (2004) Quantum connectionism and the emergence of cognition. In: Globus GG, Pribram KH, Vitiello G (eds) Brain and being: at the boundary between science, philosophy, language and arts. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 129–148Google Scholar
  239. 239.
    Bjorne P, Balkenius C (2005) A model of attentional impairments in autism: first steps toward a computational theory. Cogn Syst Res 6(3):193–204Google Scholar
  240. 240.
    Phillips AT, Wellman HM, Spelke ES (2002) Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cogn 85:53–78Google Scholar
  241. 241.
    Piaget J (1950) Introduction à l'épistémologie génétique; I La pensée mathématique. – II La pensée physique. – III La pensée biologique, la pensée psychologique et la pensée sociologique. Presses Universitaires de France,ParisGoogle Scholar
  242. 242.
    Pincus D, Guastello SJ (2005) Nonlinear Dynamics and Interpersonal Correlates of Verbal Turn‐Taking Patterns in a Group Therapy Session. Small Group Res 36:635–677Google Scholar
  243. 243.
    Plomin R (2004) Genetics and developmental psychology. Merrill‐Palmer Quarterly 50(3):341–352Google Scholar
  244. 244.
    Plomin R (2007) Genetics and developmental psychology. Wayne State University Press, DetroitGoogle Scholar
  245. 245.
    Plomin R, Asbury K (2005) Nature and nurture: genetic and environmental influences on behavior. Ann Am Acad Political Soc Sci 600:86–98Google Scholar
  246. 246.
    Plomin R, Kovas Y (2005) Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psycholog Bull 131(4):592–617Google Scholar
  247. 247.
    Popper KR (1976) Unended quest: an intellectual autobiography. Fontana, LondonGoogle Scholar
  248. 248.
    Prince CG, Hollich GJ (2005) Synching models with infants: a perceptual‐level model of infant audio‐visual synchrony detection. Cogn Syst Res 6(3):205–228Google Scholar
  249. 249.
    Raczynski S (2006) A self‐destruction game. Nonlin Dyn Psychol Life Sci 10(4):471–483Google Scholar
  250. 250.
    Richardson K (2004) Systems theory and complexity: Part 1. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(3):75–79Google Scholar
  251. 251.
    Rieber RW, Carton AS (eds) (1987) The collected works of LS Vygotsky: vol I Problems of general psychology. Plenum, New York, pp 37–285Google Scholar
  252. 252.
    Ripa J, Ives AR (2003) Food web dynamics in correlated and autocorrelated environments, Theor Popul Biol 64(3):369–384Google Scholar
  253. 253.
    Robertson D (2005) Review of agent-based modeling toolkits. Acad Manag Learn Educ 4(4):525–527Google Scholar
  254. 254.
    Robinson BF, Mervis CB (1998) Disentangling early language development: modeling lexical and grammatical acquisition using and extension of case-study methodology. Dev Psychol 34:363–375Google Scholar
  255. 255.
    Rogoff B (1990) Apprenticeship in thinking. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  256. 256.
    Ronald A, Happé F, Plomin R (2006) Genetic research into autism. Sci 311(5763):952–952Google Scholar
  257. 257.
    Rose SP, Fischer KW (1998) Models and rulers in dynamical development. Brit J Dev Psychol 16:123–131Google Scholar
  258. 258.
    Rubino C (2002) The consolations of uncertainty: time, change, and complexity. Emerg 4(1/2):200–206Google Scholar
  259. 259.
    Ruhland R, van Geert P (1998) Jumping into syntax. Brit J Dev Psychol 16:65–95Google Scholar
  260. 260.
    Rushton JP (1975) Generosity in children: immediate and long-term effects of modeling, preaching, and moral judgment. J Personal Soc Psychol 31:459–466Google Scholar
  261. 261.
    Rutter M (2007) Gene‐environment interdependence. Dev Sci 10:1–12MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  262. 262.
    Sallach D (2003) Social theory and agent architectures: prospective issues in rapid‐discovery social science. Soc Sci Comput Rev 21:179–195Google Scholar
  263. 263.
    Sameroff AJ (1975) Transactional models in early social relations. Human Dev 18(1):65–79Google Scholar
  264. 264.
    Sameroff AJ (2000) Developmental systems and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 12:297–312Google Scholar
  265. 265.
    Sameroff AJ, MacKenzie M (2003) Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: the limits of the possible. Dev Psychopathol 15(3):613–640Google Scholar
  266. 266.
    Saxe R, Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo‐parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43:1391–1399Google Scholar
  267. 267.
    Schlesinger M (2003) A lesson from robotics: modeling infants as autonomous agents. Adapt Behav 11(2):97–107MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  268. 268.
    Schlesinger M, Casey P (2003) Where infants look when impossible things happen: simulating and testing a gaze-direction model. Conn Sci 15(4):271–280Google Scholar
  269. 269.
    Schlesinger M, Parisi D (2001) The agent-based approach: a new direction for computational models of development. Dev Rev 21(1):121–146Google Scholar
  270. 270.
    Schlindwein S, Ison R (2004) Human knowing and perceived complexity: implications for systems practice. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(3):27–32Google Scholar
  271. 271.
    Schöner G, Dineva E (2007) Dynamic instabilities as mechanisms for emergence. Dev Sci 10(1):69–74Google Scholar
  272. 272.
    Schöner G, Thelen E (2006) Using dynamic field theory to rethink infant habituation. Psychol Rev 113:273–299Google Scholar
  273. 273.
    Schutte AR, Spencer JP (2002) Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: three-year-olds' delay- and experience‐dependent location memory biases. Child Dev 73:377–404Google Scholar
  274. 274.
    Schutte AR, Spencer JP, Schöner G (2003) Testing the dynamic field theory: working memory for locations becomes more spatially precise over development. Child Dev 74(5):1393–1417Google Scholar
  275. 275.
    Sebeok TA (2001) Biosemiotics: its roots, proliferation, and prospects. Semiot 134:61–78Google Scholar
  276. 276.
    Seifert KL, Hoffnung RJ (1991) Child and adolescent development, 2nd edn. Houghton, Mifflin and Company,BostonGoogle Scholar
  277. 277.
    Shahan T, Podlesnik C (2007) Divided attention and the matching law: sample duration affects sensitivity to reinforcement allocation. Learn Behav 35(3):141–148Google Scholar
  278. 278.
    Shriver M, Kramer J (1997) Application of the generalized matching law for description of student behavior in the classroom. J Behav Educ 7(2):131–149Google Scholar
  279. 279.
    Siegler RS (1983) Information processing approaches to development. In: Mussen PH (ed) Recent advances in cognitive developmental theory. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  280. 280.
    Sigelman CK, Rider EA (2006) Life-span human development, 5th edN. Thomson Wadsworth, BelmontGoogle Scholar
  281. 281.
    Simon H (1962) The architecture of complexity. Proc Am Philos Soc ISSN 0003–049X 106(6):467–482Google Scholar
  282. 282.
    Simon H, Cilliers P (2005) The architecture of complexity. Emerg: Complex Organ, 7(3/4), 138–154Google Scholar
  283. 283.
    Skinner BF (1938) The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. Appleton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  284. 284.
    Skinner BF (1945) The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychol Rev :52:270–77Google Scholar
  285. 285.
    Skinner BF (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychol Rev 57:193–216Google Scholar
  286. 286.
    Skinner C, Robinson S, Johns G, Logan P, Belfiore P (1996) Applying Herrnstein's matching law to influence students' choice to complete difficult academic tasks. J Exp Educ 65(1):5–17Google Scholar
  287. 287.
    Smith L (2005, September) Cognition as a dynamic system: Principles from embodiment. Dev Rev 25(3):278–298Google Scholar
  288. 288.
    Smith E, Conrey F (2007) Agent-based modeling: a new approach for theory building in social psychology. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 11(1):1–18zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  289. 289.
    Smith K (2002) Natural selection and cultural selection in the evolution of communication. Adapt Behav 10(1):25–44Google Scholar
  290. 290.
    Smith K, Kirby S, Brighton H (2003) Iterated learning: a framework for the emergence of language. Artif Life 9:371—386Google Scholar
  291. 291.
    Smith LB (1999) Not ‘either’, not ‘or’, not ‘both’. Dev Sci 2(2):162–163Google Scholar
  292. 292.
    Smith LB, Thelen E, Titzer R, McLin D (1999) Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychol Rev 106:235–260Google Scholar
  293. 293.
    Smith LB, Breazeal C (2007) The dynamic lift of developmental process. Dev Sci 10(1):61–68Google Scholar
  294. 294.
    Spear L (2000, August) Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 9(4):111–114Google Scholar
  295. 295.
    Spelke E, Kinzler K (2007) Core knowledge. Dev Sci 10(1):89–96Google Scholar
  296. 296.
    Spencer JP, Schöner G (2003) Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Dev Sci 4(6):392–412Google Scholar
  297. 297.
    Spencer JP, Smith LB, Thelen E (2001) Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of two-year-olds. Child Dev 72(5):1327–1346Google Scholar
  298. 298.
    Stanislas D (2007) A few steps toward a science of mental life. Mind Brain Educ 1(1):28–47Google Scholar
  299. 299.
    Steenbeek H (2006) Modeling dyadic child‐peer interactions: sociometric status, emotional expressions and instrumental actions during play. Doctoral Dissertation, University of GroningenGoogle Scholar
  300. 300.
    Steenbeek H, van Geert P (2005) A dynamic systems model of dyadic interaction during play of two children. Eur J Dev Psychol 2(2):105–145Google Scholar
  301. 301.
    Steenbeek H, van Geert P (2007) Do you still like to play with him? Variability and the dynamic nature of children's sociometric ratings. Neth J Psychol 63(3):86–101Google Scholar
  302. 302.
    Steenbeek H, van Geert P (2007) The empirical validation of a dynamic systems model of interaction; do children of different sociometric statuses differ in their dyadic play interactions? Dev Sci 11(2):253–281Google Scholar
  303. 303.
    Steenbeek H, van Geert P (2008) A dynamic systems approach to dyadic interaction in children: emotional expression, action, dyadic play, and sociometric status. Dev Rev 27(1):1–40Google Scholar
  304. 304.
    Sterman J (1994) Learning in and about complex systems. Syst Dyn Rev 10(2):291–330Google Scholar
  305. 305.
    Strauss S, Stavy R (1982) U-shaped behavioral growth. Academic, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  306. 306.
    Striano T, Henning A, Stahl D (2006) Sensitivity to interpersonal timing at 3 and 6 months of age. Interact Stud 7(2):251–271Google Scholar
  307. 307.
    Striano T, Henning A, Vaish A (2006) Selective looking by 12-month-olds to a temporally contingent partner. Interact Stud 7(2):233–250Google Scholar
  308. 308.
    Sulis W (2004) Archetypal dynamical systems and semantic frames in vertical and horizontal emergence. Emerg: Complex Organ 6(3):52–64Google Scholar
  309. 309.
    Sun R (2001) Cognitive science meets multi-agent systems: a prolegomenon. Philos Psychol 14:5–28Google Scholar
  310. 310.
    Taptiklis T (2005) After managerialism. Emerg: Complex Organ 7(3/4):2–14Google Scholar
  311. 311.
    Thagard P (1996) Mind: introduction to cognitive science. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  312. 312.
    Thelen E, Fisher D (1982) Newborn stepping: an explanation for a ‘disappearing’ reflex. Dev Psychol 18(5):760–775Google Scholar
  313. 313.
    Thelen E, Schöner G, Scheier C, Smith L (2001) The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Behav Brain Sci 24(1):1–86Google Scholar
  314. 314.
    Thelen E, Smith L (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  315. 315.
    Thelen E, Whitmyer V (2005) Using dynamic field theory to conceptualize the interface of perception, cognition, and action. In: Action as an organizer of learning and development, vol 33 in the Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 243–280Google Scholar
  316. 316.
    Thom R (1990) Semiophysics: a sketch, Addison‐Wesley, Redwood CityGoogle Scholar
  317. 317.
    Thomae H (1959) Entwicklungsbegriff und Entwicklungstheorie. In: Bergius R, Thomae H (eds) Handbuch der Psychologie, Band 3: Entwicklungspsychologie. Verlag für Psychologie, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  318. 318.
    Thomas A, Chess S (1977) Temperament and development. Brunner/Mazel, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  319. 319.
    Thomas M, Johnson M (2006) The computational modeling of sensitive periods. Dev Psychobiol 48(4):337–344Google Scholar
  320. 320.
    Thompson DE, Russell J (2004) The ghost condition: imitation versus emulation in young children's observational learning. Dev Psychol 40:882–889Google Scholar
  321. 321.
    Thompson R (2001) Sensitive periods in attachment? In: Critical thinking about critical periods. Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, pp 83–106Google Scholar
  322. 322.
    Tschacher W, Haken H (2007) Intentionality in non‐equilibrium systems? The functional aspects of self‐organized pattern formation. New Ideas Psychol 25:1–15Google Scholar
  323. 323.
    Uylings H (2006) Development of the human cortex and the concept of ‘critical’ or ‘sensitive’ periods. Lang Learn 56(1):59–90Google Scholar
  324. 324.
    Valsiner J (1987) Culture and the development of children's action: A cultural‐historical theory of developmental psychology. Wiley, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  325. 325.
    van der Maas HL (1993) Catastrophe analysis of stagewise cognitive development. Faculty of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam (Academic dissertation)Google Scholar
  326. 326.
    van der Maas H, Jansen B, Raijmakers M (2004) Developmental patterns in proportional reasoning. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  327. 327.
    van der Maas HL, Molenaar PC (1992) Stagewise cognitive development: An application of catastrophe theory. Psychol Rev 99:395–417Google Scholar
  328. 328.
    van der Veer R, Valsiner J (1991) Understanding Vygotsky: a quest for synthesis. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  329. 329.
    van Dijk M, van Geert P (2007) Wobbles, humps and sudden jumps: a case study of continuity, discontinuity and variability in early language development. Infant Child Dev 16(1):7–33Google Scholar
  330. 330.
    van Geert P (1986) The concept of development. In: van Geert P (ed) Theory building in developmental psychology. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 3–50Google Scholar
  331. 331.
    van Geert P (1987) Developmental theories as cognitive systems. Cogn Syst 2:5–39Google Scholar
  332. 332.
    van Geert P (1987) The concept of development and the structure of developmental theories. In: Baker WJ, Hyland ME (eds) Current issues in theoretical psychology: selected/edited proceedings of the founding conference of the International Society for Theoretical Psychology held in Plymouth UK, 30 August–2 September 1985. North‐Holland, Oxford, pp 379–392Google Scholar
  333. 333.
    van Geert P (1987) The structure of developmental theories. A generative approach. Hum Dev 30(3):160–177Google Scholar
  334. 334.
    van Geert P (1987) The structure of Erikson's model of the Eight Ages of Man. A generative approach. Hum Dev 30(5):236–254Google Scholar
  335. 335.
    van Geert P (1987) The structure of Galperin's theory of the formation of mental acts: a generative approach. Human Dev 30(6):355–381Google Scholar
  336. 336.
    van Geert P (1988) A graph theoretical approach to the structure of developmental models. Human Dev 31(2):107 135Google Scholar
  337. 337.
    van Geert P (1991) A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychol Rev 98:3–53Google Scholar
  338. 338.
    van Geert P (1993) A dynamic systems model of cognitive growth: competition and support under limited resource conditions. In: Smith LB, Thelen E (eds) A dynamic systems approach to development: applications. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 265–331Google Scholar
  339. 339.
    van Geert P (1994) Dynamic systems of development. Change between complexity and chaos. Harvester, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  340. 340.
    van Geert P (1994) Vygotskian dynamics of development. Human Dev 37:346–365Google Scholar
  341. 341.
    van Geert P (1995) Dimensions of change: a semantic and mathematical analysis of learning and development. Human Dev 38:322–331Google Scholar
  342. 342.
    van Geert PLC (1996) The development of attachment and attachment‐related competences. A dynamic model. In: Koops W, Hoelsma J, van den Boom J (eds) New developments in attachment research. North Holland, pp 181–199Google Scholar
  343. 343.
    van Geert P (1998) A dynamic systems model of basic developmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky and beyond. Psychol Rev 105(4):634–677Google Scholar
  344. 344.
    van Geert P (2000) The dynamics of general developmental mechanisms: from Piaget and Vygotsky to dynamic systems models. Current Direct Psychol Sci 9(2):64–68Google Scholar
  345. 345.
    van Geert P (2003) Dynamic systems approaches and modeling of developmental processes. In: Valsiner J, Conolly KJ (eds) Handbook of developmental Psychology. Sage, London, pp 640–672Google Scholar
  346. 346.
    van Geert P (2007) The Dynamic Systems approach in the study of L1 and L2 acquisition: an introduction. Mod Lang J 91:179–199Google Scholar
  347. 347.
    van Geert P (2007) Transmission, self‐organization and the emergence of language: a dynamic systems point of view. In: Schönpflug U (ed) Cultural transmission: developmental, psychological, social, and methodological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK (in press)Google Scholar
  348. 348.
    van Geert P (2008) Nonlinear‐Complex‐Dynamic‐Systems in developmental psychology. In: Guastello S, Koopmans M, Pincus D (eds) Chaos and Complexity Now and in the Future: Recent Advances in the Theory of Nonlinear Dynamical Systems Psychology. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  349. 349.
    van Geert P (2008) The Dynamic Systems approach in the study of L1 and L2 acquisition: an introduction. Mod Lang J 92(2):179–199Google Scholar
  350. 350.
    van Geert P, Fischer KW (2008). Dynamic systems and the quest forindividual‐based models of change and development. In: Spencer JP, Thomas MSC, McClelland J (eds) Toward a new grand theory of development? Connectionism and dynamic systems theory re‐considered. Oxford University Press, Oxford (in press)Google Scholar
  351. 351.
    vanGeert PLC, Savelsbergh G, van der Maas H (1999) Transitions andnon‐linear dynamics in developmental psychology. In SavelsberghG, Maas H, van Geert PLC (eds) Non‐linear developmentalprocesses. Elsevier Science Publishers, NewYorkGoogle Scholar
  352. 352.
    van Geert P, Steenbeek H (2005) The dynamics of scaffolding. New Ideas Psychol23(3):115–128Google Scholar
  353. 353.
    van Geert P, Steenbeek H (2008) Brains and the dynamics of ‘wants’ and ‘cans’:a commentary on Immordino‐Yang's “A tale of twocases”. Mind Brain Educ 2(2):62–66Google Scholar
  354. 354.
    vanOrden GC, Holden JG, Turvey MT (2005) Human Cognition and 1/fScaling. J Exp Psychol Gen134:117–123Google Scholar
  355. 355.
    Vargha‐Khadem F, Carr LJ, Isaacs E, Brett E (1997) Onset of speech after lefthemispherectomy in a nine-year-old boy. Brain: J Neurol 120:159–182Google Scholar
  356. 356.
    Vasta R, Haith MM, Miller SA (1995) Child psychology: The modern science, 2nd edn.Wiley, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  357. 357.
    Viger C (2007) The acquired language of thought hypothesis: a theory of symbolgrounding. Interact Stud 8(1):125–142Google Scholar
  358. 358.
    Vogt P, Divina F (2007) Social symbol grounding and language evolution. Interact Stud8(1):31–52Google Scholar
  359. 359.
    Vygotsky LS (1978) Cole M, John V‐Steiner, Scribner S, Souberman E (eds) Mind insociety: the development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  360. 360.
    Vygotsky LS (1986) Kozulin A (ed) Thought and language. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  361. 361.
    Vygotsky LS (1987) Thinking and speech. In: Rieber RW, Carton AS (eds) The collectedworks of LS Vygotsky: vol I Problems of general psychology. Plenum, New York, pp 37–285Google Scholar
  362. 362.
    Waldrop MM (1992) Complexity: The emerging science at the edge of order andchaos. Penguin, LondonGoogle Scholar
  363. 363.
    Weaver W (1948) Science and complexity, Am Sci 36:536–544Google Scholar
  364. 364.
    Weber-Fox C, Neville H (2001) Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- andclosed‐class words: an ERP study of bilinguals. J Speech Lang Hear Res 44(6):1338–1353Google Scholar
  365. 365.
    Weisstein EW (1999) CRC concise encyclopedia of mathematics. Chapman Hall/CRC, BocaRatonGoogle Scholar
  366. 366.
    Wertsch JV (1985) Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Harvard University Press,CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  367. 367.
    Wertsch JV (1991) Voices of the mind: a sociocultural approach to mediatedaction. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  368. 368.
    Westermann G, Mareschal D, Johnson M, Sirois S, Spratling M, Thomas M (2007)Neuroconstructivism. Dev Sci 10(1):75–83Google Scholar
  369. 369.
    Wildgen W (1986) Processual semantics of the verb. J Semant 5(4):321–344Google Scholar
  370. 370.
    Wildgen W (1982) Catastrophe theoretic semantics-: an elaboration andapplication of René Thom's theory. Benjamins,AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  371. 371.
    WilliamsJHG, Waiter GD, Gilchrist A, Perrett DI, Murray AD, Whiten A (2006)Neural mechanisms of imitation and ‘mirror neuron’functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia44:610–621Google Scholar
  372. 372.
    WimmersRH, Beek PJ, Van Wieringen PC (1992) Phase transitions inrhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling.Hum Mov Sci 11:217–226Google Scholar
  373. 373.
    WimmersRH, Savelsbergh GJP, Beek PJ, Hopkins B (1998) Evidencefor a phase transition in the early development ofprehension. Dev Psychobiol32:235–248Google Scholar
  374. 374.
    WimmersRH, Savelsbergh GJP, van der Kamp J, Hartelman P (1998b)A developmental transition in prehension modeled as a cuspcatastrophe. Dev Psychobiol32:23–35Google Scholar
  375. 375.
    Worgan S, Damper R (2007) Grounding symbols in the physics of speechcommunication. Interact Stud 8(1):7–30Google Scholar
  376. 376.
    Yavuz H (2007) An integrated approach to the conceptual design and development of anintelligent autonomous mobile robot. Robot Autonom Syst 55(6):498–512Google Scholar
  377. 377.
    Yoshikawa Y, Asada M, Hosoda K, Koga J (2003) A constructivist approach to infants'vowel acquisition through mother‐infant interaction. Connect Sci 15(4):245–258Google Scholar

Books and Reviews

  1. 378.
    JansenBRJ, van der Maas HLJ (2001a) Evidence for the Phase transition fromrule I to rule II on the balance scale task. Dev Rev 21:450–494Google Scholar
  2. 379.
    JansenBRJ, van der Maas HLJ (2002a) The development of children's rule useon the balance scale task. J Exp Child Psychol81:383–416Google Scholar
  3. 380.
    vander Maas HLJ, Jansen BRJ (2003) What response times tell of children'sbehavior on the balance scale task. J Exp Child Psychol85:141–177Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Paul van Geert
    • 1
  1. 1.The Heymans InstituteGroningenThe Netherlands