Advertisement

Cultural Cognition as a Conception of the Cultural Theory of Risk

  • Dan M. Kahan

Abstract

Cultural cognition is one of a variety of approaches designed to empirically test the “cultural theory of risk” set forth by Mary Douglas and Aaron Wildavsky. The basic premise of cultural theory is that individuals can be expected to form beliefs about societal dangers that reflect and reinforce their commitments to one or another idealized form of social ordering. Among the features of cultural cognition that make it distinctive among conceptions of cultural theory are its approach to measuring individuals’ cultural worldviews; its empirical investigation of the social psychological mechanisms that connect individuals’ risk perceptions to their cultural worldviews; and its practical goal of enabling self-conscious management of popular risk perceptions in the interest of promoting scientifically sound public policies that are congenial to persons of diverse outlooks.

Keywords

Risk Perception Cultural Cognition Cultural Predisposition Cultural Theory Collective Management 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

Research described herein was supported by the National Science Foundation (Grants SES-0621840 & SES-0242106) and the Oscar M. Ruebhausen Fund at Yale Law School.

References

  1. Aiken LS, West SG, Reno RR (1991) Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Sage, Newbury ParkGoogle Scholar
  2. Balkin JM (1998) Cultural Software. Yale Univ. Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  3. Boudon R (1998) Social mechanisms without black boxes. In: Hedström P, Swedberg R (eds) Social mechanisms: an analytical approach to social theory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 172–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Braman D, Kahan DM, Grimmelmann J (2005) Modeling facts, culture, and cognition in the gun debate. Soc J Res 18:283–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cohen GL (2003) Party over policy: the dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. J Pers Soc Psychol 85(5):808–822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cohen GL, Aronson J, Steele CM (2000) When beliefs yield to evidence: reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 26(9):1151–1164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cohen GL, Bastardi A, Sherman DK, Hsu L, McGoey M, Ross L (2007) Bridging the partisan divide: self-affirmation reduces ideological closed-mindedness and inflexibility in negotiation. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(3):415–430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cultural Cognition Project (2007) The second national risk and culture study: Making sense of - and making progress in - the American culture war of fact. Yale Law School, New Haven, Conn. October 2007, available at http://www.culturalcognition.net/projects/second-national-risk-culture-study.html
  9. Dake K (1990) Technology on trial: orienting dispositions toward environmental and health standards. Ph.D. dissertation. University of California at Berkeley, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  10. Dake K (1991) Orienting dispositions in the perception of risk: an analysis of contemporary worldviews and cultural biases. J Cross Cult Psychol 22:61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dake K (1992) Myths of nature: culture and the social construction of risk. J Soc Issues 48(4):21–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Deuteronomy 28:22 (1997). In New American Standard BibleGoogle Scholar
  13. Douglas M (1966) Purity and danger: an analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. Routledge, LondonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Douglas M (1970) Natural symbols: explorations in cosmology. Barrie & Rockliff the Cresset Press, LondonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Douglas M (1982) In the active voice. Routledge & K. Paul, London/BostonGoogle Scholar
  16. Douglas M (1985) Risk acceptability according to the social sciences. Russell Sage, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Douglas M (1986) How Institutions Think, 1st edn. Syracuse University Press, SyracuseGoogle Scholar
  18. Douglas M (1992) Risk and blame: essays in cultural theory. Routledge, London/New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Douglas M (1997) The depoliticization of risk. In: Ellis RJ, Thompson M (eds) Culture matters: essays in honor of Aaron Wildavsky. Westview Press, Boulder, pp 121–132Google Scholar
  20. Douglas M (2003) Being fair to hierarchists. Univ Penn Law Rev 151(4):1349–1370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Douglas M, Wildavsky AB (1982) Risk and culture: an essay on the selection of technical and environmental dangers. University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  22. Ellis RJ, Thompson F (1997) Culture and the environment in the Pacific Northwest. Am Polit Sci Rev 91(4):885–898CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Elster J (1985) Making sense of Marx. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  24. Finucane M, Slovic P, Mertz CK, Flynn J, Satterfield TA (2000) Gender, race, and perceived risk: the “white male” effect. Health Risk Soc'y 3(2):159–172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gastil J, Jenkins-Smith H, Silva C (1995) Analysis of cultural bias survey items (Institute for Public Policy, University of New Mexico, 1995)Google Scholar
  26. Giner-Sorolla R, Chaiken S (1997) Selective use of heuristic and systematic processing under defense motivation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 23(1):84–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gross JL, Rayner S (1985) Measuring culture: a paradigm for the analysis of social organization. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  28. Jenkins-Smith H (2001) Modeling stigma: an empirical analysis of nuclear waste images of Nevada. In: Flynn J, Slovic P, Kunreuther H (eds) Risk, media, and stigma: understanding public challenges to modern science and technology. Earthscan, London/Sterling, pp 107–132Google Scholar
  29. Jenkins-Smith HC, Herron KG (2009) Rock and a hard place: public willingness to trade civil rights and liberties for greater security. Polit Policy 37(5):1095–1129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Judd CM (2000) Everyday data analysis in social psychology: comparisons of linear models. In: Reis HT, Judd CM (eds) Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 370–392Google Scholar
  31. Kahan DM (2007) The cognitively illiberal state. Stan L Rev 60:115–154Google Scholar
  32. Kahan DM (2010) Emotion in risk regulation: competing theories. In: Roeser S (ed) Emotions and risky technologies. Springer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  33. Kahan DM, Braman D (2003a) Caught in the crossfire: a defense of the cultural theory of gun-risk perceptions - response. Univ Penn Law Rev 151(4):1395–1416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kahan DM, Braman D (2003b) More statistics, less persuasion: a cultural theory of gun-risk perceptions. Univ Pann Law Rev 151:1291–1327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kahan DM, Braman D (2006) Cultural cognition of public policy. Yale J L & Pub Pol'y 24:147–170Google Scholar
  36. Kahan DM, Slovic P, Braman D, Gastil J (2006) Fear of democracy: a cultural critique of sunstein on risk. Harv Law Rev 119:1071–1109Google Scholar
  37. Kahan DM, Braman D, Gastil J, Slovic P, Mertz CK (2007) Culture and identity-protective cognition: explaining the white-male effect in risk perception. J Empirical Legal Stud 4(3):465–505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kahan DM, Braman D, Slovic P, Gastil J, Cohen G (2009) Cultural cognition of the risks and benefits of Nanotechnology. Nat Nanotechnol 4(2):87–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kahan DM, Braman D, Cohen GL, Gastil J, Slovic P (2010a) Who fears the HPV vaccine, who doesn't, and why? An experimental study of the mechanisms of cultural cognition. Law Hum Behav 34:501–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kahan DM, Braman D, Monahan J, Callahan L, Peters E (2010b) Cultural cognition and public policy: the case of outpatient commitment laws. Law Hum Behav 34:118–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kahan DM, Jenkins-Smith HC, Braman D (2011) Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. J Risk Res 14:147–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1982) Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. In: Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A (eds) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge/New York, pp 163–178Google Scholar
  43. Langford IH, Georgiou S, Bateman IJ, Day RJ, Turner RK (2000) Public perceptions of health risks from polluted coastal bathing waters: a mixed methodological analysis using cultural theory. Risk Anal 20(5):691–704CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lord CG, Ross L, Lepper MR (1979) Biased assimilation and attitude polarization – effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 37(11):2098–2109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mamadouh V (1999) Grid-group cultural theory: an introduction. GeoJournal 47:395–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Manton KG, Woodbury MA, Stallard E, Corder LS (1992) The use of grade-of-membership techniques to estimate regression relationships. Sociol Methodol 22:321–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Marris C, Langford IH, O'Riordan T (1998) A quantitative test of the cultural theory of risk perceptions: comparison with the psychometric paradigm. Risk Anal 18(5):635–647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Nisbett RE (2003) The geography of thought: how Asians and Westerners think differently–and why. Free Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  49. O'Connor RE, Bord RJ, Fisher A (1998) Rating threat mitigators: faith in experts, governments, and individuals themselves to create a safer world. Risk Anal 18(5):547–556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Peters E, Slovic P (1996) The role of affect and worldviews as orienting dispositions in the perception and acceptance of nuclear power. J Appl Soc Psychol 26(16):1427–1453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rayner S (1992) Cultural theory and risk analysis. In: Krimsky S, Golding D (eds) Social theories of risk. Praeger, Westport, pp 83–115Google Scholar
  52. Silva CL, Jenkins-Smith HC (2007) The precautionary principle in context: U.S. and E.U. scientists' prescriptions for policy in the face of uncertainty. Soc Sci Q 88(3):640–664CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Slovic, P (2000) The Perception of Risk. Earthscan Publications, London/Sterling, VAGoogle Scholar
  54. Sjöberg L (1998a) Explaining risk perception: an empirical evaluation of cultural theory. In: Löfstedt RE, Frewer L (eds) The earthscan reader in risk and modern society, vol 2. Earthscan, London, pp 115–132Google Scholar
  55. Sjöberg L (1998b) World views, political attitudes, and risk perception. Risk Health Saf Environ 9:137–152Google Scholar
  56. Thompson M, Ellis R, Wildavsky A (1990) Cultural theory. Westview Press, BoulderGoogle Scholar
  57. Verweij M, Thompson M (eds) (2006) Clumsy solutions for a complex world: governance, politics, and plural perceptions. Palgrave Macmillan, Houndmills/Basingstoke/Hampshire/New YorkGoogle Scholar
  58. Verweij M, Douglas M, Ellis R, Engel C, Hendriks F, Lohmann S et al (2006) Clumsy solutions for a complex world: the case of climate change. Public Admin 84(4):817–843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wildavsky AB (1991) The rise of radical egalitarianism. American University Press, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  60. Wildavsky A, Dake K (1990) Theories of risk perception: who fears what and why? Daedalus 114:41–60Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Yale Law SchoolYale UniversityNew HavenUSA

Personalised recommendations