Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology

2018 Edition
| Editors: Jeffrey S. Kreutzer, John DeLuca, Bruce Caplan

Metacognition

  • Sean CunninghamEmail author
  • Zheng Zhou
  • Candace Hughes
  • Jacqueline Estrada
Reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57111-9_1464

Synonyms

Cognitive awareness; Meta-knowing; Thinking-about-thinking

Short Description or Definition

Metacognition refers to awareness and knowledge of one’s own thoughts and cognitive processes. Metacognition refers also to the ability to monitor, regulate, and control thoughts.

Categorization

Memory process, Neuropsychological process, Metamemory.

Natural History, Prognostic Factors, Outcomes

Historical Background

Metacognition developed out of research from developmental and cognitive psychology on awareness and monitoring of memory, referred to as metamemory (Flavell 1971, 1979). The accuracy of judgments that people make about their memory is what Hart referred to as “feelings of knowing” (Hart 1965, 1967). Developmental psychologists, such as Flavell, have been concerned with discovering if the emerging conscious understanding of the processes of memory and cognition relates to memory improvement in children. From these early studies, interest in metamemory spreads to other areas...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References and Readings

  1. Ardila, A. (2008). On the evolutionary origins of executive functions. Brain and Cognition, 68, 92–99.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Babinski, J. (1914). Contribution a l’etude des troubles mentaux dans l’hemiplegie orga-nique cerebral (Anosognosie). [Contribution to the study of mental disturbance in organic cerebral hemiplegia. (Anosognosia)]. Revue Neurologique, 27, 845–848.Google Scholar
  3. Baker, L. (1994). Fostering metacognitive development. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 25, 201–239.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown, A. L. (1975). The development of memory: Knowing, knowing about knowing and knowing how to know. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 103–152). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  5. Brown, A. L. (1978). Knowing when, where and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advanced in instructional psychology (pp. 77–165). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  6. Brown, T. M., Ris, M. D., Beebe, D., Ammerman, R. T., Oppenheimer, S. G., Yeates, K. O., et al. (2008). Factors of biological risk and reserve associated with executive behaviors in children and adolescents with spina bifida myelomeningocele. Child Neuropsychology, 14, 118–134.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Fernandez-Duque, D., & Black, S. E. (2007). Metacognitive judgment and denial of deficit: Evidence from frontotemporal dementia. Judgment and Decision Making, 2, 359–370.Google Scholar
  8. Flavell, J. H. (1971). Stage-related properties of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 421–453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. D. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 19(367), 1338–1349.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Li, S. J., & Stein, E. A. (2000). A parametric manipulation of central executive functioning. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 585–592.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Gioia, G., Isquith, P., & Guy, S. (2001). Assessment of executive functions in children with neurological impairment. In R. Simeonsson & S. Rosenthal (Eds.), Psychological and developmental assessment: Children with disabilities and chronic conditions (pp. 317–356). New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
  13. Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 208–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Hoffmann, M., & Schmitt, F. (2003). Cognitive impairment in isolated subtentorial stroke. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 109, 14–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kuhn, D. (1999). A developmental model of critical thinking. Educational Researcher, 28, 16–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kuhn, D. (2004). Metacognitive development. In J. Lerner & A. E. Alberts (Eds.), Current directions in developmental psychology (pp. 55–60). Upper Saddle River: Pearson.Google Scholar
  17. Luria, A. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  18. Luria, A. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Meijer, J., Veenman, M., & van Hout-Wolters, B. (2006). Metacognitive activities in text-studying and problem-solving: Development of a taxonomy. Educational Research and Evaluation, 12, 209–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Middleton, F. A., & Strick, P. L. (2001). Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex in the primate. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 700–712.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Milner, B. (1995). Aspects of human frontal lobe functions. In H. Jasper, S. Riggio, & P. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.), Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the frontal lobe (pp. 67–84). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  22. Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: View from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220–246.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Osaka, N. (2007). The frontal lobe correlates of metacognition: Neural interpretation based on the cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Japanese Psychological Review, 50, 216–226.Google Scholar
  25. Paris, S. G. (2002). When is metacognition helpful, debilitating, or benign? In P. Chambres, M. Izaute, & P. Marescaux (Eds.), Metacognition: Process, function and use (pp. 105–120). Norwell: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Paris, S. G., & Winograd, P. W. (1990). How metacognition can promote academic learning and instruction. In B. J. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.), Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction (pp. 15–51). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  27. Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337–344.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Prigatano, G. P. (1991). Disturbances of self-awareness of deficit after traumatic brain injury. In G. P. Prigatano & D. L. Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of deficit after brain injury: Clinical and theoretical issues (pp. 111–126). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stuss, D., & Knight, R. (Eds.). (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Wells, A. (2000). Emotional disorders and metacognition. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sean Cunningham
    • 1
    Email author
  • Zheng Zhou
    • 2
  • Candace Hughes
    • 3
  • Jacqueline Estrada
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Educational PsychologyUniversity of UtahSalt Lake CityUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologySt. John’s UniversityQueensUSA
  3. 3.School of PsychologyThe Chicago School of Professional PsychologyChicagoUSA