Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Cognitive Map

Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_899-1

The notion of cognitive map has a more general meaning as a metaphor for relations in the experiences of life and a more specific meaning in terms of cognition of relations among spatial attributes. The general term was made famous by the American learning theorist Edward Tolman (1886–1959), who formulated the term cognitive map to mean cognition of relational aspects of experience (Tolman 1948). This term has seen a revival in the last decade, as a region of the brain intimately connected with spatial cognition, the hippocampus, was found to code another fundamental dimension of experience, that of time. The spatial meaning of the term cognitive map received a boost with the discovery of neurons in the hippocampus of rats that fire the most when the rat was at a particular place in its environment, an arena in a lab. This discovery of what are called place cellsis attributed to behavioral neuroscientist John O’Keefe. The spatial notion, discussed most in rats, humans, and honeybees,...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access

References

  1. Benhamou, S. (1996). No evidence for cognitive mapping in rats. Animal Behaviour, 52, 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Calhoun, J. B. (1963). The ecology and sociology of the Norway rat. Bethesda: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23, 149–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cheng, K., & Jeffery, K. J. (2017). Spatial cognition. In J. Call (Ed.), APA handbook of comparative psychology. Vol. 2: Perception, learning, and cognition (pp. 463–483). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Menzel, R., Greggers, U., Smith, A., Berger, S., Brandt, R., Brunke, S., et al. (2005). Honeybees navigate according to a map-like spatial memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 3040–3045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  8. Putman, N. F., Endres, C. S., Lohmann, C. M. F., & Lohmann, K. J. (2011). Longitude perception and bicoordinate magnetic maps in sea turtles. Current Biology, 21, 463–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Schiller, D., Eichenbaum, H., Buffalo, E. A., Davachi, L., Foster, D. J., Leutgeb, S., & Ranganath, C. (2015). Memory and space: Towards an understanding of the cognitive map. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 13904–13911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Singer, R. A., Abroms, B. D., & Zentall, T. R. (2006). Formation of a simple cognitive map by rats. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 19, 417–425.Google Scholar
  11. Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Warren, W. H., Rothman, D. B., Schnapp, B. H., & Ericson, J. D. (2017). Wormholes in virtual space: From cognitive maps to cognitive graphs. Cognition, 166, 152–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Biological SciencesMacquarie UniversitySydneyAustralia

Section editors and affiliations

  • Lauren Guillette
    • 1
  1. 1.University of St. AndrewsSt. AndrewsUK