Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Pointing

  • Adam A. Pack
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_753-1

Introduction

An early developmental hallmark in human social cognition is the ability to understand pointing as a form of referential communication. Referential pointing in humans is a triadic communicative transaction involving the management of joint attention between an informant, a receiver, and a subject of interest. Pointing is not only adaptive as a form of communication but it can also inform a receiver and an informant about each other’s visual perception, social awareness, and knowledge state. In human infants, the ability to understand the referring function of the pointing cue develops over the first 2 years of life (reviewed in Pfandler et al. 2013). Since the early 1990s, there has been an “explosion” of studies in nonhuman animals on how they respond to pointing cues, whether they produce pointing cues, and if they appreciate a pointing cues’ referring function (see summaries in Miklosi and Soproni 2006, Pack and Herman 2006; Lyn 2010; Kaminski and Nitzschner 2013;...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 359–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1994). Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 307–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. D’Aniello, R., Alterisio, A., Scandurra, A., Petremolo, E., Iommelli, M. R., & Aria, M. (2017). What’s the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand pointing gestures. Animal Cognition, 20, 777–787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Essler, J. L., Schwartz, L. P., Rossettie, M. S., & Judge, P. G. (2017). Capuchin monkeys’ use of human and conspecific cues to solve a hidden object-choice task. Animal Cognition, 20, 985–998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Giret, N., Miklosi, A., Kreutzer, M., & Bovet, D. (2009). Use of experimenter-given cues by African gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Animal Cognition, 12, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Halina, M., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The goal of ape pointing. PLoS One, 13(4), e0195182.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195182.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Hall, N. J., Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., Walsh, A. L., & Wynne, C. D. (2011). Megachiropteran bats (Pteropus) utilize human referential stimuli to locate hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 341–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Communication of food location between human and dog (Canis familiaris). Evolution and Communication, 2, 137–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68, 571–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R., & Trut, L. (2005). Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Current Biology, 15, 226–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Itakura, S., & Tanaka, M. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 119–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Itakura, S., Agnetta, B., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Chimpanzee use of human and conspecific social cues to locate food. Developmental Science, 2, 448–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Herman, L. M., Abichandani, S. L., Elhajj, A. N., Herman, E. Y. K., Sanchez, J. L., & Pack, A. A. (1999). Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referential character of the human pointing gesture. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 347–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Herman, L. M., Pack, A. A., & Morrel-Samuels, P. (1993). Conceptual and representational abilities in bottlenosed dolphins. In H. R. Roitblat, L. M. Herman, & P. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and communication: Comparative Perspectives. (pp. 403–442). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Kaminski, J., & Nitszchner, M. (2013). Do dogs get the point? A review of dog-human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 44, 294–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69, 11–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lakatos, G., Soproni, K., Doka, A., & Miklosi, A. (2009). A comparative approach to dogs’ (Canis familiaris) and human infants’ comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures. Animal Cognition, 12, 621–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lakatos, G., Gacsi, M., Topal, J., & Miklosi, A. (2012). Comprehension and utilization of pointing gestures and gazing in dog-human communication in relatively complex situations. Animal Cognition, 15, 201–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lyn, H. (2010). Environment, methodology, and the object choice task in apes: Evidence for declarative comprehension and implications for the evolution of language. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 333–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lyn, H., Russell, J. L., & Hopkins, W. D. (2010). The impact of environment on the comprehension of declarative communication in apes. Psychological Science, 21, 360–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Malassis, R., & Delfour, F. (2015). Sea lions’ (Zalophus californianus) use of human pointing gestures as referential cues. Learning & Behavior, 43, 101–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Maros, K., Gacsi, M., & Miklosi, A. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus). Animal Cognition, 11, 457–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. McKinley, J., & Sambrook, T. D. (2000). Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus). Animal Cognition, 3, 13–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Meunier, H., Prieuer, J., & Vauclair, J. (2013). Olive baboons communicate intentionally by pointing. Animal Cognition, 16, 155–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1, 113–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Miklosi, A., & Soproni, K. (2006). A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Animal Cognition, 9, 81–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2009). The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object-choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 304–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nawroth, C., Ebersbach, M., & von Borell, E. (2014). Juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domestica) use human-given cues in an object choice task. Animal Cognition, 17, 701–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (2004). Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referent of both static and dynamic human gazing and pointing in an object choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 160–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (2006). Dolphin social cognition: Our current understanding. Aquatic Mammals, 32, 443–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (2007). The dolphin’s (Tursiops truncatus) understanding of human gazing and pointing: Knowing what and where. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 34–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Peignot, P., & Anderson, J. R. (1999). Use of experimenter-given manual and facial cues by gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) in an object-choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 253–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pfandler, E., Lakatos, G., & Miklosi, A. (2013). Eighteen-month-old human infants show intenstive development in comprehension of different types of pointing gestures. Animal Cognition, 16, 711–719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Povinelli, D. J., Reaux, J. E., Bierschwale, D. T., Allain, A. D., & Simon, B. B. (1997). Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but no adolescent chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 12, 423–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Proops, L., Walton, M., & McComb, K. (2010). The use of human-given cues by domestic horses, Equus caballus, during an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 79, 1205–1209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Scheumann, M., & Call, J. (2004). The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Animal Cognition, 7, 224–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Shapiro, A. D., Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. B. (2003). Gray seal (Halichoerus grypus) pup responses to experimental-given pointing and directional cues. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 355–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Smet, A. F., & Byrne, R. W. (2013). African elephants can use human pointing cues to find hidden food. Current Biology, 23, 2033–2037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Smet, A. F., & Byrne, R. W. (2014). Interpretation of human pointing by African elephants: Generalization and rationality. Animal Cognition, 17, 1365–1374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2002). Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116, 27–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tauzin, T., Csik, A., Kis, A., & Topal, J. (2015). What or where? The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129, 334–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origin and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  43. Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Gluckman, A. (1997). Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Development, 68, 1067–1080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tschudin, A., Call, J., Dunbar, R. I. M., Harris, G., & van der Elst, C. (2001). Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 100–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2008). Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1767–1773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Viranyi, Z., Gasci, M., Kubinyi, E., Topal, J., Belenyi, B., Ujfalussy, D., & Miklosi, A. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 11, 373–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Xitco, M. J., Gory, J. D., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2001). Spontaneous pointing by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Cognition, 4, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Xitco, M. J., Gory, J. D., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2004). Dolphin pointing is linked to the attention behaviour of a receiver. Animal Cognition, 7, 231–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Departments of Psychology and BiologyUniversity of Hawaii at HiloHiloUSA
  2. 2.The Dolphin InstituteHiloUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Sarah Dunphy-Lelii
    • 1
  1. 1.Bard CollegeAnnandale-On-HudsonUSA