Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Food Calls

  • Julie Gros-LouisEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1664-1

Synonyms

Definition

Vocalizations that are produced while foraging and/or feeding.

Food calls, also known as food-associated calls, occur in several animal species and are emitted upon discovering food and during foraging/feeding bouts (primates: Cebus capucinus, Boinski and Campbell 1996; Cebus apella, Di Bitetti 2003; Ateles geoffroyi, Chapman and Lefebvre 1990; Saguinus oedipus, Cleveland and Snowdon 1982; Roush and Snowdon 2000; Saguinus labiatus, Caine et al. 1995; Callithrix jacchus, Vitale et al. 2003; Callithrix geoffroyi, Kitzman and Caine 2009; Leontopithecus rosalia, Benz et al. 1992; Cebuella pygmaea, Pola and Snowdon 1975; Macaca mulatta, Hauser and Marler 1993; Macaca sinica, Dittus 1984; Pan troglodytes, Hauser et al. 1993; Wrangham 1977; Pan paniscus, van Krunkelsven et al. 1996; Gorilla g. gorilla, Luef et al. 2016; naked mole rats: Heterocephalus glaber, Judd and Sherman 1996; dolphins: Tursiops...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Benz, J. J. (1993). Food-elicited vocalizations in golden-lion tamarins: Design features for representational communication. Animal Behaviour, 45, 443–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Benz, J. J., Leger, D. W., & French, J. A. (1992). The relation between food preference and food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 142–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Boinski, S., & Campbell, A. F. (1996). The “huh” vocalization of white-faced capuchins – A spacing call disguised as a food call. Ethology, 102, 826–840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown, C. R., Brown, M. B., & Shaffer, M. L. (1991). Food-sharing signals among socially foraging cliff swallows. Animal Behaviour, 42, 551–564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bugnyar, T., Kijne, M., & Kotrschal, K. (2001). Food calling in ravens: Are yells referential signals? Animal Behaviour, 61, 949–958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Caine, N. G., Addington, R. L., & Windfelder, T. L. (1995). Factors affecting the rates of food calls given by red-bellied tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 50, 53–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carazo, P., & Font, E. (2010). Putting information back into biological communication. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 23, 661–669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chapman, C. A., & Lefebvre, L. (1990). Manipulating foraging group size: Spider monkey food calls at fruiting trees. Animal Behaviour, 39, 891–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clarke, J. A. (2010). White-tailed ptarmigan food calls enhance chick diet choice: Learning nutritional wisdom? Animal Behaviour, 79, 25–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clay, Z., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2009). Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos. Animal Behaviour, 77, 1387–1396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clay, Z., Smith, C. L., & Blumstein, D. T. (2012). Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: What do these calls really mean? Animal Behaviour, 83, 323–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cleveland, J., & Snowdon, C. T. (1982). The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 58, 231–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Di Bitetti, M. S. (2003). Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkey (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour, 140, 565–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Di Bitetti, M. S. (2005). Food-associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Animal Behaviour, 69, 911–919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Di Bitetti, M. S., & Janson, C. H. (2001). Social foraging and the finder’s share in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Behaviour, 62, 47–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dittus, W. P. J. (1984). Toque macaque food calls: Semantic communication concerning food distribution in the environment. Animal Behaviour, 32, 470–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Elgar, M. A. (1986a). House sparrows establish foraging flocks by giving chirrup calls if the resources are divisible. Animal Behaviour, 34, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Elgar, M. A. (1986b). The establishment of foraging flocks in house sparrows: Risk of predation and daily temperature. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 19, 433–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Elowson, A. M., Tannenbaum, P. L., & Snowdon, C. T. (1991). Food associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 42, 931–937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Evans, C. S., & Evans, L. (1999). Chicken food calls are functionally referential. Animal Behaviour, 58, 307–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Evans, C. S., & Marler, P. (1994). Food calling and audience effects in male chickens, Gallus gallus: Their relationships to food availability, courtship and social facilitation. Animal Behaviour, 47, 1159–1170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fedurek, P., & Slocombe, K. E. (2013). The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 726–739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gros-Louis, J. (2004a). The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) from the perspective of the signaller. Animal Behaviour, 67, 431–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gros-Louis, J. (2004b). White-faced capuchins’ responses to naturalistic and experimentally presented food-associated calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 396–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hauser, M. D., & Marler, P. (1993). Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behavioral Ecology, 4, 206–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hauser, M. D., Teixidor, P., Field, L., & Flaherty, R. (1993). Food-elicited calls in chimpanzees: Effects of food quantity and divisibility. Animal Behaviour, 45, 817–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Heinrich, B. (1988). Winter foraging at carcasses by three sympatric corvids, with emphasis on recruitment by the raven, Corvus corax. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 23, 141–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Heinrich, B., & Marzluff, J. M. (1991). Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 28, 13–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Janik, V. M. (2000). Food-related bray calls in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 267, 923–927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Judd, T. M., & Sherman, P. W. (1996). Naked mole-rats recruit colony mates to food sources. Animal Behaviour, 52, 957–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kalan, A. M., & Boesch, C. (2015). Audience effects in chimpanzee food calls and their potential for recruiting others. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69, 1701–1712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kalan, A. M., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2015). Wild chimpanzees modify food call structure with respect to tree size for a particular fruit species. Animal Behaviour, 101, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kitzman, C. D., & Caine, N. G. (2009). Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) food-associated calls are functionally referential. Ethology, 115, 439–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Luef, E. M., Breuer, T., & Pika, S. (2016). Food-associated calling in gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla) in the wild. PLoS One, 11(2), e0144197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Macedonia, J. M., & Evans, C. S. (1993). Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology, 93, 177–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Marler, P., Dufty, A., & Pickert, R. (1986a). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken: I. Does a sender communicate information about the quality of a food referent? Animal Behaviour, 34, 188–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Marler, P., Dufty, A., & Pickert, R. (1986b). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken: II. Is a sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver? Animal Behaviour, 34, 194–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Newman, J. A., & Caraco, T. (1989). Co-operative and non-cooperative bases of food calling. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 141, 197–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Perry, S. (1996). Female-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. American Journal of Primatology, 40, 167–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Perry, S. (1997). Male-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour, 134, 477–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pizzari, T. (2003). Food, vigilance, and sperm: The role of male direct benefits in the evolution of female preference in a polygamous bird. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 593–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pola, Y. V., & Snowdon, C. T. (1975). The vocalizations of pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea. Animal Behaviour, 23, 826–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Radford, A. N., & Ridley, A. R. (2008). Close calling regulates spacing between foraging competitors in the group-living pied babbler. Animal Behaviour, 75, 519–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., & Ryan, M. J. (2009). What do animal signals mean? Animal Behaviour, 78, 233–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Robinson, J. G. (1982). Vocal systems regulating within-group spacing. In C. T. Snowdon, C. H. Brown, & M. R. Petersen (Eds.), Primate communication (pp. 94–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Roush, R. S., & Snowdon, C. T. (2000). Quality, quantity, distribution and audience effects on food calling in cotton-top tamarins. Ethology, 106, 673–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schel, A. M., Machanda, Z., Townsend, S. W., Zuberbuhler, Z., & Slocombe, K. E. (2013). Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Animal Behaviour, 86, 955–965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2010). Animal communication: Insights from linguistic pragmatics. Animal Behaviour, 79, e1–e4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 145–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Bergman, T., Fischer, J., Zuberbühler, K., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2010). The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Animal Behaviour, 80, 3–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberhubler, K. (2006). Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: Responses to food types or food preferences? Animal Behaviour, 72, 989–999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Valone, T. J. (1996). Food-associated calls as public information about patch quality. Oikos, 77, 153–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. van Krunkelsven, E., Dupain, J., Van Elsacker, L., & Verheyen, R. F. (1996). Food calling in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): An experiment. International Journal of Primatology, 17, 207–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Vitale, A., Zanzoni, M., Queyras, A., & Chiarotti, F. (2003). Degree of social contact affects the emission of food calls in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 59, 21–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wauters, A. M., & Richard-Yris, M. A. (2002). Mutual influence of the maternal hen’s food calling and feeding behavior on the behavior of her chicks. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, 25–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wheeler, B. C., & Fischer, J. (2012). Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21, 195–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wilkinson, G. S., & Boughman, J. W. (1998). Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear-nosed bats. Animal Behaviour, 55, 337–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wrangham, R. W. (1977). Feeding behaviour of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In T. H. Clutton-Brock (Ed.), Primate ecology: Studies of feeding and ranging behaviour in lemurs, monkeys and apes (pp. 503–538). London: Academic.Google Scholar
  59. Wright, G. S. (2014). Social calls predict foraging success in big brown bats. Current Biology, 24, 885–889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesUniversity of IowaIowa CityUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Shannon Digweed
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMacEwan UniversityEdmontonCanada