Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Chemical Signals

  • Jason N. BruckEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1654-1

Synonyms

Related

Pheromones, Chemical cues, Olfactory cues, Gustatory cues, Kin-recognition, MHC, Arm-pit effect, Self-referent phenotype matching

Definition

Chemical signals can be defined as messages transmitted through chemosensory modalities (smell and taste) and include things like pheromones, which are secreted chemical signals used to trigger a response in another individual. This section includes chemical communication with little effort to distinguish between cues and signals. For some, the difference between a cue and a signal relates to intentionality or whether the communication is incidental (Maynard-Smith and Harper 2003). For others, costs and benefits to the sender and the receiver help make the determination between signals and cues, and this is tied closely to evolutionary analysis (Laidre and Johnstone 2013). Therefore, I will review the diversity of messages possible and the unique aspects of gustation and olfaction as...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bland, K. P., & Jubilan, B. M. (1987). Correlation of flehmen by male sheep with female behavior and oestrus. Animal Behaviour, 35, 735–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Burton, R. (1976). The language of smell. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd..Google Scholar
  3. Caldwell, R. L. (1985). A test of individual recognition in the stomatopod Gonodactylus festae. Animal Behaviour, 33, 101–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cornu, J.-N., Cancel-Tassin, G., Ondet, V., Girardet, C., & Cussenot, O. (2011). Olfactory detection of prostate cancer by dogs sniffing urine: A step forward in early diagnosis. European Urology, 59(2), 197–201.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eururo.2010.10.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Cozzi, B., Huggenberger, S., & Oelschläger, H. (2016). Anatomy of dolphins (1st ed.). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  6. Crosland, H. R., Goodman, M., & Hockett, A. (1926). Anosmia and its effects upon taste perceptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 398–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dunbar, I. F. (1977). Olfactory preferences in dogs: The response of male and female beagles to conspecific odors. Behavioral Biology, 20(4), 471–481.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-6773(77)91079-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Feng, P., Zheng, J., Rossiter, S. J., Wang, D., & Zhao, H. (2014). Massive losses of taste receptor genes in toothed and baleen whales. Genome Biology and Evolution, 6, 1254–1265.  https://doi.org/10.1093/gbe/evu095.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hardie, J., Nottingham, S. F., Powell, W., & Wadhams, L. J. (1991). Synthetic aphid sex pheromone lures female parasitoids. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 61, 97–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hyman, A., Mentzer, T., & Calderone, L. (1979). The contribution of olfaction to taste discrimination. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 13(6), 359–362.Google Scholar
  12. Laidre, M. E., & Johnstone, R. A. (2013). Animal signals. Current Biology, 23(18), R829–R833.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.070.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Lawson, R. E., Putman, R. J., & Fielding, A. H. (2001). Chemical communication in Eurasian deer (Cervidae): Do individual odours also code for attributes? Journal of Zoology, 253(1), 91–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Linsenmair, K. E. (1987). Kin recognition in subsocial arthropods, in particular in the desert isopods Hemilepistus reaumuri. In D. J. C. Fletcher & C. D. Michener (Eds.), Kin recognition in animals (pp. 121–208). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Mackay Sim, A., & Laing, D. G. (1980). Discrimination of odors from stressed rats by non-stressed rats. Physiology and Behavior, 24(4), 699–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Mateo, J. M. (2003). Kin recognition in ground squirrels and other rodents. Journal of Mammalogy, 84, 1163–1181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mateo, J. M. (2010). Self-referent phenotype matching and long-term maintenance of kin recognition. Animal Behaviour, 80, 929–935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mateo, J. M., & Johnston, R. E. (2000). Kin recognition and the ‘armpit effect’: Evidence of self-referent phenotype matching. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 267(1444), 695–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Mateo, J. M., & Johnston, R. E. (2003). Kin recognition by self-referent phenotype matching: Weighing the evidence. Animal Cognition, 6, 73–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Maynard-Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Mead, K. S., & Caldwell, R. L. (2011). Mantis shrimp: Olfactory apparatus and chemosensory behavior. In T. Breihaupt & M. Thiel (Eds.), Chemical communication in crustaceans (pp. 219–238). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  22. Mueller-Schwarze, D. (1979). Flehmen in the context of mammalian urine communication. Chemical ecology: Odour communication in animals. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical Press.Google Scholar
  23. Nachtigall, P. E. (1986). Vision, audition, and chemoreception in dolphins and other marine mammals. In R. J. Schusterman, J. A. Thomas, & F. G. Wood (Eds.), Dolphin cognition and behavior: A comparative approach (pp. 79–113). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  24. Sayigh, L. S., Tyack, P. L., Wells, R. S., Solow, A. R., Scott, M. D., & Irvine, A. B. (1998). Individual recognition in wild bottlenose dolphins: A field test using playback experiments. Animal Behaviour, 57, 41–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Schaefer, M. L., Yamazaki, K., Osada, K., Restrepo, D., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2002). Olfactory fingerprints for major histocompatibility complex-determined body odors II: Relationship among odor maps, genetics, odor composition, and behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(21), 9513–9521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Sherman, P. W. (1977). Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science, 197(4310), 1246–1253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Swaisgood, R. R., Lindburg, D. G., & Zhang, H. (2002). Discrimination of oestrous status in giant pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) via chemical cues in urine. Journal of Zoology, 257(3), 381–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Symonds, M. R., Johnson, T. L., & Elgar, M. A. (2012). Pheromone production, male abundance, body size, and the evolution of elaborate antennae in moths. Ecology and Evolution, 2(1), 227–246.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.81.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Thom, M. D., & Hurst, J. L. (2004). Individual recognition by scent. Annales Zoologici Fennici, 41(6), 765–787.Google Scholar
  30. Tibbetts, E. A., & Dale, J. (2007). Individual recognition: It is good to be different. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22(10), 529–537.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2007.09.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Todrank, J., & Heth, G. (1996). Individual odours in two chromosomal species of blind, subterranean mole rat (Spalax ehrenbergi): Conspecific and cross-species discrimination. Ethology, 102, 806–811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Todrank, J., Heth, G., & Johnston, R. E. (1998). Kin recognition in golden hamsters: Evidence for kinship odours. Animal Behaviour, 55(2), 377–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Vetter, K. M., & Caldwell, R. (2015). Individual recognition in stomatopods. In L. Aquiloni & E. Tricarico (Eds.), Social recognition in invertebrates: The knowns and the unknowns (p. 266). Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
  34. Zenuto, R. R., & Fanjul, M. S. (2002). Olfactory discrimination of individual scents in the subterranean rodent Ctenomys talarum (tuco-tuco). Ethology, 108(7), 629–641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Zuk, M., & Kolluru, G. R. (1998). Explotations of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 73, 415–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Integrative BiologyOklahoma State UniversityStillwaterUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Shannon Digweed
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMacEwan UniversityEdmontonCanada