Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

Behavior Systems

  • Robert Ian BowersEmail author
Living reference work entry

Latest version View entry history

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1232-2

Beginnings in Ethology

Nikolaas Tinbergen ( 1942) began to speak in terms of systems of behavior in his first manifesto of ethology. As there are systems of respiration and systems of digestion, for instance, in which multiple chores are accomplished in coordination by different parts to perform a complex function, there can be said to be systems of behavior that collectively accomplish complex chores such as feeding, finding mates, avoiding pain, and perhaps less obvious functions as well, such as play, grooming, or sociality. Tinbergen ( 1942) stressed that behavior has an organization and that this organization is hierarchical (Fig. 1).
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Akins, C. A., Domjan, M., & Gutiérrez, G. (1994). Topography of sexually conditioned behavior in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) depends on the CS–US interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 199–209.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baerends, G. P. (1941). On the life history of Ammophila campestris. Jur. Nederl. Akademie van Wetenschappen, 44, 483–488.Google Scholar
  3. Baerends, G. P. (1976). The functional organization of behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 24, 726–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bindra, D. (1961). Components of general activity and the analysis of behavior. Psychological Review, 68, 205–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brooks, R. A. (1986). A robust layered control system for a mobile robot. IEEE Journal of Robotics and Automation, RA-2, 14–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burghardt, G. M., & Bowers, R. I. (2017). From instinct to behavior systems: An integrated approach to ethological psychology. In J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. Pepperberg, C. Snowdon, & T. Zentall (Eds.), APA handbook of comparative psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 333–364). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  7. Craig, W. (1918). Appetites and aversions as constituents of instincts. Biological Bulletin, 34, 91–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Domjan, M. P. (1994). Formulation of a behavior system for sexual conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 421–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 429–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hogan, J. A. (1994). Structure and development of behavior systems. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 439–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  12. Silva, F. J., Timberlake, W., & Cevik, M. O. (1998a). A behavior systems approach to the expression of backward associations. Learning and Motivation, 29, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Silva, F. J., Timberlake, W., & Gont, R. S. (1998b). Spatiotemporal characteristics of serial CSs and their relation to search modes and response form. Animal Learning & Behavior, 26, 299–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Timberlake, W. (1983a). The functional organization of appetitive behavior: Behavior systems and learning. In M. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behaviour (Vol. 3, pp. 177–221). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Timberlake, W. (1983b). Rats responses to a moving object related to food or water: A behavior systems analysis. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 309–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Timberlake, W. (1990). Natural learning in laboratory paradigms. In D. A. Dewsbury (Ed.), Contemporary issues in comparative psychology (pp. 31–54). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Timberlake, W. (1994). Behavior systems, associationism, and Pavlovian conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 405–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Timberlake, W. (2001). Motivational modes in behavior systems. In R. R. Mowrer & S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp. 155–209). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Timberlake, W. (2007). Anthropomorphism revisited. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 2, 139–144.Google Scholar
  20. Timberlake, W., & Grant, D. L. (1975). Auto-shaping in rats to the presentation of another rat predicting food. Science, 190, 690–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Timberlake, W., Wahl, G., & King, D. (1982). Stimulus and response contingencies in the misbehavior of rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 62–85.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Tinbergen, N. (1942). An objectivistic study of the innate behaviour of animals. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
  23. Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of MinhoGualtarPortugal

Section editors and affiliations

  • Oskar Pineno
    • 1
  1. 1.Hofstra UniversityLong IslandUSA