Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Art Production, Appreciation and Fitness

  • Michelle Scalise SugiyamaEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3793-1

Synonyms

Definition

“Line, color, pattern, and/or form used by humans to modify an object/body solely to attract attention to that object/body” (Coe 2003, p. 173).

Introduction

Although there are hints of earlier art activity (Malotki and Dissanayake 2018), the oldest, uncontentious evidence of art behavior consists of shell beads and engraved ochre dated to roughly 75 ka (Henshilwood et al. 2002). This tells us that art behaviors emerged in a hunting-and-gathering context and thus are not an outgrowth of conditions associated with “civilization” – agriculture, permanent settlements, high population densities, social stratification, personal wealth, and economic specialization. Their presence in forager societies makes these species-typical behaviors (Brown 1991) particularly puzzling, because they impose considerable time and energy costs yet appear to yield no fitness benefits. Consequently, opinion is...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Barbeau, M. (1929). Totem poles of the Gitksan, Upper Skeena River, British Columbia. Bulletin of the National Museum of Canada, No. 61. Ottawa: F. A. Acland.Google Scholar
  2. Barrett, H. C. (2005). Adaptations to predators and prey. In D. Buss, The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken: John Wiley and Sons, 200–223.Google Scholar
  3. Barrett, H. C., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). The hominid entry into the cognitive niche. In S. Gangestad and J. Simpson, Evolution of Mind: Fundamental Questions and Controversies. New York: Guilford Press, 241–248.Google Scholar
  4. Bogoras, W. (1904–1909). The Chukchee. The Jesup North Pacific Expedition, Vol 7. New York: American Museum of Natural History.Google Scholar
  5. Brand, R. J., & Shallcross, W. L. (2008). Infants prefer motionese to adult-directed action. Developmental Science, 11(6), 853–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown, D. (1991). Human Universals. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Chagnon, N. (1997). Yanomamö. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  8. Chatterjee, A. (2014). The aesthetic brain: How we evolved to desire beauty and enjoy art. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Coe, K. (2003). The Ancestress Hypothesis: Visual Art as Adaptation. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1988). The biological foundation of aesthetics. In Rentschler, I., Herzberger, B., and Epstein, D., Beauty and the Brain: Biological Aspects of Aesthetics. Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag, 29–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ficken, M. S. (1977). Avian play. The Auk, 94(3), 573–582.Google Scholar
  13. Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition, 100(1), 173–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hagen, E. H., & Bryant, G. A. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature, 14(1), 21–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hagen, E. H., & Hammerstein, P. (2009). Did Neanderthals and other early humans sing? Seeking the biological roots of music in the territorial advertisements of primates, lions, hyenas, and wolves. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2_suppl), 291–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, G. A., et al. (2002). Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science, 295(5558), 1278–1280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Jochim, M. (1983). Palaeolithic cave art in ecological perspective. In G. Bailey, Hunter-Gatherer Economy in Prehistory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 212–219.Google Scholar
  18. Kellogg, R. (1969). Analyzing children’s art. Palo Alto: Mayfield Pub Co.Google Scholar
  19. Krebs, J., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In J. Krebs and N. Davies, Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, 2nd ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, 380–402.Google Scholar
  20. Lee, R. B. (1984). The Dobe! Kung. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  21. Malotki, E., & Dissanayake, E. (2018). Early rock art of the American West: The geometric enigma. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
  22. Marshack, A. (1972). Cognitive aspects of Upper Paleolithic engraving. Current Anthropology, 13(3/4), 445–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Marshall, L. (1976). Sharing, talking, and giving: Relief of social tensions among !Kung Bushmen. In N. Blurton Jones and M. Konner, Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 349–371.Google Scholar
  24. Mendoza, M. (2007). Human trophy taking in the South American Gran Chaco. In R. Chacon and D. Dye, The Taking and Displaying of Human Body Parts as Trophies by Amerindians. Boston: Springer, 575–590.Google Scholar
  25. Miller, G. F. (1999). Sexual selection for cultural displays. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, and C. Power, The Evolution of Culture: An Interdisciplinary View. Rutgers University Press, 71–91.Google Scholar
  26. Mithen, S. J., & Mithen, S. R. (1990). Thoughtful Foragers: A Study of Prehistoric Decision Making. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Morris, D. (2013). The Artistic Ape: Three Million Years of Art. London: Red Lemon Press.Google Scholar
  28. Mountford, C., & Tonkinson, R. (1969). Carved and engraved human figures from north Western Australia. Anthropological Forum, 2(3), 370–390.Google Scholar
  29. Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby, The Adapted Mind. New York: Oxford University Press, 555–579.Google Scholar
  30. Owsley, D. W., Bruwelheide, K. S., Burgess, L. E., & Billeck, W. T. (2007). Human finger and hand bone necklaces from the Plains and Great Basin. In R. Chacon and D. Dye, The Taking and Displaying of Human Body Parts as Trophies by Amerindians. Boston: Springer, 124–166.Google Scholar
  31. Pardoe, C. (1995). Riverine, biological and cultural evolution in southeastern Australia. Antiquity, 69(265), 696–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Parkman, E. B. (1986). Cupule petroglyphs in the Diablo Range, California. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology, 8(2), 0191–3557.Google Scholar
  33. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  34. Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W. (1999). The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(6–7), 15–51.Google Scholar
  35. Rasmussen, K. (1929). Intellectual Culture of the Iglulik Eskimos. Report of the Fifth Thule Expedition, 1921–1924.Google Scholar
  36. Ray, V. F. (1938). Lower Chinook ethnographic notes. University of Washington Publications in Anthropology, 7(2), 29–165.Google Scholar
  37. Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017). Oral storytelling as evidence of pedagogy in forager societies. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sugiyama, L. S. (2016). Physical attractiveness: An adaptationist perspective. In D. Buss, The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 2nd ed. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 371–434.Google Scholar
  39. Thomas, E. M. (2006). The Old Way: A Story of the First People. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
  40. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Does beauty build adapted minds? Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction, and the arts. SubStance, 30(1), 6–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Turnbull, C. M. (1983). The Mbuti Pygmies: Change and Adaptation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  42. Verpooten, J., & Nelissen, M. (2010). Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: The late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory in Biosciences, 129(2–3), 211–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Zoeller, M., Horowitz, E., German, T., & Cosmides, L. (2018). Morphological cues of animacy: Saggital plane symmetry supercedes face and whole-target visibility in predicting the speed of superordinate level classification. Poster presented at the annual meetings of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, Amsterdam.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of OregonEugeneUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Guilherme S. Lopes
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyOakland UniversityRochesterUSA