Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Vocal Communication

  • Gregory A. Bryant
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3352-1

Synonyms

Definition

Vocal communication is the transfer of information through the auditory channel via voice production mechanisms.

Introduction

Vocal communication is ubiquitous in mammal species. As a mode of signaling, vocalizations often manifest in alarm call systems, territorial displays, and courtship rituals and are particularly well-suited for functions requiring wide broadcast. But vocal signals are used in an incredible variety of communicative contexts, many of which are extremely quiet, and between small groups of animals. Here I will describe two fundamental issues important for the study of evolution of vocal communication in humans and nonhuman animals: the distinction between adaptive signals and byproduct cues, and the concept of form and function in the structure of animal signals. I will then apply these principles to current evolutionary research on vocal behavior in humans specifically.

In...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Ackermann, H., Hage, S. R., & Ziegler, W. (2014). Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: An evolutionary perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(6), 529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bryant, G. A. (2013). Animal signals and emotion in music: Coordinating affect across groups. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(990), 1–13.Google Scholar
  3. Bryant, G. A., Fessler, D. M. T., Fusaroli, R., et al. (2016). Detecting affiliation in colaughter across 24 societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(17), 4682–4687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391–428). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Filippi, P., Congdon, J. V., Hoang, J., Bowling, D. L., Reber, S. A., Pašukonis, A., ... Newen, A. (2017). Humans recognize emotional arousal in vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates: Evidence for acoustic universals. Proceedings of Royal Society B, 284(1859), 20170990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 258–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Maynard-Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Owren, M. J., & Rendall, D. (2001). Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 10(2), 58–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Pisanski, K., & Bryant, G. A. (2018). The evolution of voice perception. In N. S. Eidsheim & K. L. Meizel (Eds.), Oxford handbook of voice studies. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Pisanski, K., Cartei, V., McGettigan, C., Raine, J., & Reby, D. (2016). Voice modulation: A window into the origins of human vocal control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 304–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Scott-Phillips, T. (2014). Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. London: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Communication, Center for Behavior, Evolution, and CultureUniversity of California, Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Christopher D. Watkins
    • 1
  1. 1.Division of Psychology, School of Social and Health SciencesAbertay UniversityDundeeUK