Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Nonhuman Tool Use

  • Ivo Jacobs
  • Mathias Osvath
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3152-1

Synonyms

Definition

The external employment of an unattached or manipulable attached environmental object to alter more efficiently the form, position, or condition of another object, another organism, or the user itself, when the user holds and directly manipulates the tool during or prior to use and is responsible for the proper and effective orientation of the tool. (Shumaker et al. 2011, p. 5)

Introduction

Nonhuman tool use has long been a source of much fascination. Claims of human uniqueness have often revolved around the ability to use and manufacture tools. When Jane Goodall discovered the extent of tool use by wild chimpanzees in the 1960s, Louis Leakey commented that “now we must redefine tool, redefine man, or accept chimpanzees as humans” (Goodall 1998, p. 2184). After countless observations of animals using and manufacturing tools, it is now evident that “man the tool-maker” (Oakley 1956) is an...

Keywords

Stone Tool Capuchin Monkey Trap Tube American Crow Caledonian Crow 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bentley-Condit, V. K., & Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: Current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour, 147, 185–221. doi:10.1163/000579509X12512865686555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Birch, H. G. (1945). The relation of previous experience to insightful problem-solving. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 38, 367–383. doi:10.1037/h0056104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Call, J. (2013). Three ingredients for becoming a creative tool user. In C. M. Sanz, J. Call, & C. Boesch (Eds.), Tool use in animals: Cognition and ecology (pp. 3–20). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511894800.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Crist, E. (2002). The inner life of earthworms: Darwin’s argument and its implications. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 3–8). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. Dinets, V., Brueggen, J., & Brueggen, J. (2015). Crocodilians use tools for hunting. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 27, 74–78. doi:10.1080/03949370.2013.858276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Emery, N. J. (2013). Insight, imagination and invention: Tool understanding in a non-tool-using corvid. In C. M. Sanz, J. Call, & C. Boesch (Eds.), Tool use in animals: Cognition and ecology (pp. 67–88). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511894800.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fox, E. A., Sitompul, A. F., & van Schaik, C. P. (1999). Intelligent tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & H. L. Miles (Eds.), The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans (pp. 99–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511542305.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fragaszy, D. M., & Cummins-Sebree, S. E. (2005). Relational spatial reasoning by a nonhuman: The example of capuchin monkeys. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4, 282–306. doi:10.1177/1534582306286573.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Goodall, J. (1998). Learning from the chimpanzees: A message humans can understand. Science, 282, 2184–2185. doi:10.1126/science.282.5397.2184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Haslam, M., Hernandez-Aguilar, A., Ling, V., Carvalho, S., de la Torre, I., DeStefano, A., Du, A., Hardy, B., Harris, J., Marchant, L., Matsuzawa, T., McGrew, W., Mercader, J., Mora, R., Petraglia, M., Roche, H., Visalberghi, E., & Warren, R. (2009). Primate archaeology. Nature, 460, 339–344. doi:10.1038/nature08188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Haslam, M., Luncz, L. V., Staff, R. A., Bradshaw, F., Ottoni, E. B., & Falótico, T. (2016). Pre-Columbian monkey tools. Current Biology, 26, R521–R522. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.046.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Hunt, G. R., Gray, R. D., & Taylor, A. H. (2013). Why is tool use rare in animals? In C. M. Sanz, J. Call, & C. Boesch (Eds.), Tool use in animals: Cognition and ecology (pp. 89–118). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511894800.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Jacobs, I. F., von Bayern, A., & Osvath, M. (2016). A novel tool-use mode in animals: New Caledonian crows insert tools to transport objects. Animal Cognition, 191, 249–1252. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-1016-z.Google Scholar
  14. Jelbert, S. A., Taylor, A. H., & Gray, R. D. (2015). Investigating animal cognition with the Aesop’s fable paradigm: Current understanding and future directions. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 8, e1035846. doi:10.1080/19420889.2015.1035846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Köhler, W. (1927). The mentality of apes. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. doi:10.1037/11338-000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lambert, M. L., Seed, A. M., & Slocombe, K. E. (2015). A novel form of spontaneous tool use displayed by several captive greater vasa parrots (Coracopsis vasa). Biology Letters, 11, 20150861. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2015.0861.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Laumer, I. B., Bugnyar, T., & Auersperg, A. M. I. (2016). Flexible decision-making relative to reward quality and tool functionality in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana). Scientific Reports, 6, 28380. doi:10.1038/srep28380.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Oakley, K. (1956). The earliest tool-makers. Antiquity, 30, 4–8. doi:10.1017/s0003598x00026351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Osvath, M., & Osvath, H. (2008). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0157-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Povinelli, D. J., & Penn, D. C. (2011). Through a floppy tool darkly: Towards a conceptual overthrow of animal alchemy. In T. McCormack, C. Hoerl, & S. Butterfill (Eds.), Tool use and causal cognition (pp. 69–88). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Povinelli, D. J., Reaux, J. E., & Frey, S. H. (2010). Chimpanzees’ context-dependent tool use provides evidence for separable representations of hand and tool even during active tool use within peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 48, 243–247. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.01.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Rutz, C., & St Clair, J. (2012). The evolutionary origins and ecological context of tool use in New Caledonian crows. Behavioural Processes, 89, 153–165. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2011.11.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Rutz, C., Klump, B. C., Komarczyk, L., Leighton, R., Kramer, J., Wischnewski, S., Sugasawa, S., Morrissey, M. B., James, R., St Clair, J. J. H., Switzer, R. A., & Masuda, B. M. (2016). Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature, 537, 403–407. doi:10.1038/nature19103.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Sanz, C. M., & Morgan, D. B. (2013). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee tool use. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368, 20120416. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Seed, A. M., & Byrne, R. W. (2010). Animal tool-use. Current Biology, 20, R1032–R1039. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.042.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Seed, A. M., Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2011). Causal knowledge in corvids, primates and children: More than meets the eye? In T. McCormack, C. Hoerl, & S. Butterfill (Eds.), Tool use and causal cognition (pp. 89–110). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199571154.003.0005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R., & Beck, B. B. (2011). Animal tool behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. Baltimore: JHU Press.Google Scholar
  28. Taylor, A., Hunt, G., & Gray, R. (2012). Context-dependent tool use in New Caledonian crows. Biology Letters, 8, 205–207. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0782.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Tebbich, S., & Teschke, I. (2013). Why do woodpecker finches use tools? In C. M. Sanz, J. Call, & C. Boesch (Eds.), Tool use in animals: Cognition and ecology (pp. 134–157). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511894800.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Thorpe, W. H. (1963). Learning and instinct in animals. London: Methuen.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Lund UniversityLundSweden

Section editors and affiliations

  • Catherine Salmon
    • 1
  1. 1.University of RedlandsRedlandsUSA