Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Fighting Assessment

  • Vít TřebickýEmail author
  • Michael Stirrat
  • Jan Havlíček
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2738-1

Synonyms

Definition

Neuro-cognitive processes involved in estimating others’ and one’s own ability to inflict harm and other fitness-related costs in physical confrontation based on available cues related to the likelihood of success in physical conflict.

Introduction

When organisms are competing for the same resource there are multiple strategies that each individual might take. They might cooperate, they might try to scramble to outcompete, they might leave and seek alternative sources, or they might aggressively compete (Duntley 2005). This last choice is a decision to inflict costs on the other and is likely made where at least one of the other strategies are available. It is therefore an interesting question to analyse when and how organisms decide to aggressively compete.

While a number of factors will be at play in the wider context, such as the value of the resource and how divisible it is, for our purposes here we focus on the relative fighting abilities of the prospective combatants in a human context or as Parker (1974) describes it the decision whether to fight or not may be based upon an assessment of “relative resource holding potential.”

It is possible that assessment of the other prospective candidate may be unnecessary. Arnott and Elwood (2009) report that in a variety of species, there is evidence for a pure self-assessment where the prospective combatant pays attention to its own resources and health and will only deescalate from a fight if these resources drop below some trigger point. This strategy avoids the costly effort of evaluating others. There is some evidence for use of this reduced cost strategy in humans. Taller men (Watkins et al. 2010a) or merely individuals that feel powerful in the moment (Watkins and Jones 2012; Watkins et al. 2010b) have a reduced ability to discriminate differences in male facial dominance. This suggests that their self-assessment is sufficient to at least ignore rival characteristics although not precisely evidence that self-assessment would be enough to escalate to a fight.

What is more likely, however, is that human competitors mutually assess rivals before considering escalating to physical aggression. Various models exist to describe this but all involves initial low-cost sampling of the rival abilities before escalating (Arnott and Elwood 2009).

In many species, agonistic conflicts with conspecifics are key determinants of access to fitness contributors like resources, territory, and mating opportunities or promotion in social hierarchy (Ellis 1995). Species that engage frequently in physical confrontations with conspecifics calibrate their behavior according to their relative fighting ability. Many such species assess fighting ability from visual, auditory, or olfactory cues. Moreover, individuals remember these assessments over time, use them to determine relative fighting ability, calibrate their assessments based on previous victories or losses in fights, and engage in ritualized behavior involving displays and mutual assessments of formidability, and these contests are frequently resolved when one of the antagonists surrenders, which prevents actual costly fights (Sell et al. 2012).

Any individual that can reliably assess the chance to win a conflict in advance and that can make more sensible decisions about whether to initiate, escalate, or retreat from a potential fight is likely to obtain selective advantage. Thus, it seems likely that natural selection would have favored the evolution of cognitive and behavioral mechanisms that would facilitate assessment of fighting ability and that promote the most appropriate decisions and responses to decrease costs and increase benefits from potential confrontations.

Perception of numerous socially relevant characteristics that are important in social contexts (e.g., confrontations and negotiations) are frequently determined by physical traits like conspecifics’ morphology. In many species, direct visual inspection of others’ physical size is well documented (Arnott and Elwood 2009) as primary determinant of social dominance and formidability (see Třebický and Havlíček 2017). Individuals across various taxa can recognize the relative dominance, including fighting ability, of their conspecific upon first encounter depending on displays of body size or presence and size of armaments, ranging from paper wasps (Tibbetts and Lindsay 2008), hermit crabs (Neil 1985), monitor lizards (Frýdlová et al. 2017), numerous avian species (Fretwell 1969; Senar and Camerino 1998), domestic pigs (Rushen 1987) to non-human primates (Bergman et al. 2009; Ghazanfar and Santos 2004).

Recalibrational Theory of Anger

Fighting ability could be described as an ability to efficiently use physical strengths (among other means), against a conspecific antagonist. Greater fighting ability of the given individual should increase the costs the individual is able to inflict upon the opponent. The willingness to use ones’ fighting ability against competitors is commonly mediated via anger. Individuals are deemed to use anger to convince others to treat them better, and the more power they had to harm others, the more convincing they would have been (Price et al. 2012).

The recalibrational theory of anger (Sell 2011) is a computational evolutionary model which proposes that the function of anger is to recalibrate individuals who place insufficient weight on the welfare of the angry individual when making fight-or-flight decisions. According to this theory, (i) individuals differ in terms of their anger thresholds and selective usage of negotiation tactics, (ii) they need to estimate others’ traits relevant to the ability to inflict any costs (e.g., fighting ability) and provide any benefits (e.g., mate value or resource gathering potential), (iii) and use these estimates to calibrate their welfare trade-off ratios (Tooby et al. 2008). More weight is placed on the welfare of individuals who have higher Resource Holding Power – the abilities and willingness to inflict higher costs or appropriate benefits from others (Parker 1974; Sell 2011). Individuals with higher resource holding power easily win conflicts, attain preferential access to mates, and benefit themselves at the expense of others in other ways (Sell 2017). Individuals are expected to have evolved to calibrate their welfare trade-off ratios to maximize their own welfare. Such recalibration strategy confers important selective advantage over fighting without prior assessment.

Due to the significant role of mate selection in evolutionary theorizing, the vast majority of investigations in current behavioral sciences emphasize that intersexual selection (i.e., processes underlying mate choice) is responsible for men’s phenotype (but see Sell et al. 2017; for review Třebický et al. 2012). However, recent studies suggest that a variety of masculine traits appears to be primarily designed for intrasexual competition rather than for attraction of potential mates (Hill et al. 2013; Puts 2010; Sell et al. 2017).

Human inclinations towards antagonistic interactions have been displayed throughout our evolutionary history. Forensic evidence suggests that antagonistic interactions among our ancestors – especially among men – have been rather common and significant selection force in our ancestral environment (Manson et al. 1991; Walker 1997, 2001). Facial skeletal trauma supposedly caused by fist fighting exists in the fossil record of Australopithecus (Roper 1969) and early Homo (Wu et al. 2011) and many other samples of historic skeletal remains (Walker 1997). Historical records and ethnography literature contain numerous examples that acts of physical encounters are not exceptional in many human cultures across the globe (Archer 2009; Horns et al. 2015; Walker 2001) and ritualized combats in the form of folk wrestling and martial arts are also common and have a long history (Green 2010). As in many other vertebrate species, men show higher levels of physical aggression, engage in physical contests and homicides to members of the same sex more frequently than women, and since practically all wars were fought prevailingly by men, this potential seems to be much higher among men than women in virtually all societies worldwide (Archer 2004, 2009). These patterns suggest a relatively high level of male intrasexual physical competition in human evolutionary history. This may account for some typical features of the human male physique, especially for the upper body shape and a greater amount of muscle mass and strength compared to the female body (Lassek and Gaulin 2009). Evolutionary-informed scholars therefore argue that physical encounters have probably generated powerful selection pressures in humans as well that have contributed to the establishment of various neurocognitive and behavioral complexes, whose function is to perceive, assess, and specifically act in antagonist situations or even prior to actual encounters (Carré et al. 2013; Puts 2010; Sell et al. 2009a).

Physical Armaments

Physical competition often favors evolution of anatomical armaments (structures used as weapons or armors). Unlike some other animals, humans are not equipped with dangerous weaponries such as sharp canines, claws, or poisonous glands. Before humans started using and fabricating objects as weapons, physical combat had depended on hits executed by hands and striking with fists appears to be universally employed in many cultures across the globe as dominant striking technique used in modern combatant arts and “street fights” as well (Horns et al. 2015; Morgan and Carrier 2013; Scoggin et al. 2010). Ancestrally, a man’s upper body strength was therefore a major component of his ability to inflict costs on others. Upper-body strength is considered as critical for physical encounters, particularly for wrestling, punching, or choking, which were the most common types of combats in ancestral populations according to analyses of skeletal remains (Walker 1997). Hence, greater strength should set the individual’s formidability higher.

Interestingly, hand morphology which is similar to modern humans was present in basal hominins (Horns et al. 2015) and although, proportions of the hand may improve manual dexterity, at the same time they make it possible for the hand to clench into fist and be used as a club against opponents. According to the protective buttressing hypothesis, bones of the human hand are proportioned in a way so that they provide a supportive buttress while clenched into fist which protects the hand from injuries and also that the human fist functions effectively to strengthen and stabilize the hand during punches, which allows competing males to strike with higher force while substantively reducing the risk of hand injury (Horns et al. 2015; Morgan and Carrier 2013). Nevertheless, the frequency of hand bone fractures resulting from fighting (Jeanmonod et al. 2011) has been used to argue that the hand is too fragile as a weapon (King 2013). Importantly, in fist fights the primary target is the face (Brink et al. 1998), and bones of our faces break much more frequently than do bones of the hands during fights, indicating that the fist is a sufficiently effective weapon (Carrier and Morgan 2014).

Facial Morphology

Studies focusing on the locations of injuries that result from assaults and interpersonal violence found that the face was the most common site (Brink et al. 1998; Shepherd et al. 1990) and fractures most frequently occurred in the mandibles, nasal complex, zygoma arches, maxillae, and supraorbital arches. Interestingly, the bone structures that suffer from the highest fracture rates are the parts of the skull that exhibit the greatest increase in robusticity during the evolution of genus Homo (Carrier and Morgan 2014) and are the most sexually dimorphic parts of the human skull (Enlow et al. 1996). Overall facial shape, expansion, and bunodont form of post canine teeth, increased robusticity of the orbits, excessively stronger masticatory system are the traits that tentatively represent functional features of protective buttressing of the face against injury during fighting (Carrier and Morgan 2014). It is also likely that masculine features of the face convey direct information about the degree to which the face is vulnerable to injury. A robust facial skeleton may make an individual stronger opponent simply because he is less susceptible to knockouts and serious injury.

Understanding others can help one to forecast others’ future behavior and adjust oneself accordingly. Being able to assess mating interests, trustworthiness, willingness to cooperate, or formidability of conspecifics is of high importance for individuals’ fitness, therefore to have neuro-cognitive mechanisms that work for perceiving and responding to these characteristics and intentions would be very effective. Such mechanisms should swiftly recognize adequate cues in others relying minimally on direct interaction and should be sensitive to cues that are related to fighting ability in this instance. This is carried out by first impressions – impressions that can be formed very quickly, based on whatever information is available. Such first impressions are often formed by using, among other features, the visual appearance of physical features of one’s face. A substantial body of evidence shows that people attribute various characteristics to others based on their facial appearances like physical strength (Holzleitner and Perrett 2016; Sell et al. 2009a) and aggressive behavior (Carré et al. 2009), among others.

Converging lines of evidence show that people infer various psychological traits based on static facial features. For instance, people judged as more powerful were reported being higher in assertiveness, aggressiveness, and power (Berry 1991), those who were judged as stronger and better fighters were physically stronger (Sell et al. 2009b), fighting success was congruently assessed (Little et al. 2015; Třebický et al. 2013, 2015), and even criminals who were judged as more violent were more likely to have been confined for violent crimes (Stillman et al. 2010). Majority of research on cues of threat potential in humans has investigated the relationships between measures of men’s threat potential such as body size, upper-body strength, or actual fighting ability and assessments of their facial traits (Arnott 2017; Sell 2017; Sell et al. 2012; Třebický et al. 2013). Several studies have reported positive correlations between measures of men’s upper-body strength and ratings of their facial dominance, strength, and ascribed fighting ability (Fink et al. 2007; Holzleitner and Perrett 2016; Windhager et al. 2011). Other studies found that faces of taller men are perceived as more dominant (Burton and Rule 2013; Re et al. 2013a; Watkins et al. 2010a). Ratings from distinct cultures including hunter-horticulturalists, people from industrialized regions, and pastoralists show that cues of physical strength and aggression are present in the face and that these cues can be extracted and assessed rapidly and accurately (Sell et al. 2010; Short et al. 2012; Třebický et al. 2015; Zilioli et al. 2014). A recent study by Han et al. (2017) showed that men’s perceived “facial threat potential” (a composite measure derived from dominance, strength, and weight ratings based on their faces) was positively related to their scores on the “actual threat potential” (constructed from men’s handgrip strength, weight, and height). Man’s handgrip strength (a proxy measure of upper body strength) is positively correlated with women’s ratings of their facial dominance (controlling for age and body weight) (Fink et al. 2007). Also, perceived strength from faces is correlated with actual muscle performance (Holzleitner and Perrett 2016; Hugill et al. 2009) and peer ratings of each other’s fighting ability are tracked by rating of dominance from facial images by strangers (Doll et al. 2014).

Threat Potential May Not Directly Relate to Formidability

It should be stressed, however, that previous studies concerning assessment and measures of formidability (Han et al. 2017; Sell et al. 2009b) rely on body strength or body size as a proxy for fighting ability. While physical strength is undoubtedly an important component of fighting ability, there are many additional sources of variation in the ability to fight. These studies are rather studying a threat potential; however, even high threat potential does not need to translate into actual fighting ability. Several researchers (Dixson et al. 2017; Kraus and Chen 2013; Little et al. 2015; Palmer-Hague et al. 2015; Pollet et al. 2013; Třebický et al. 2013) have overcome this issue by using available data on fighting ability from databases of mixed martial arts contests. Nevertheless, the generalization from these data might be limited as the sample consists of elite athletes highly skilled in hand-to-hand combat and individual bouts are fought only within restricted weight classes.

In most cases of hand-to-hand confrontation in general public, little-to-no experience or formal training can be expected, and therefore other factors like overall body size that influence the outcome of a physical fight must be at play. The fighting ability is undoubtedly a complex skill affected by various characteristics (i.e., ability to concentrate in stress, hand-to-eye coordination, overall stamina, or experiences) and our knowledge in this area is far from definite and contribution of other characteristics may vary according to the form of the fight.

A growing body of research aims to identify exact facial (and bodily) morphological traits and whether people use them as visual cues to estimate those characteristics from the face and body and subsequently test how accurately perceived characteristics relate to relevant morphological structures. For example, in the case of body size, taller people generally have more elongated faces than shorter ones (Windhager et al. 2011) and heavier men have wider and squarer lower facial regions (Coetzee et al. 2010). Available evidence also indicates that people use the abovementioned or related facial cues to assess others’ body weight and height, and they are fairly accurate in their judgments (Re et al. 2013b). It was found that low brows, large chins, wide noses, and narrow mouths are morphological traits related to perceived dominance and strength in both natural and computer-generated faces (Toscano et al. 2014), and that physical strength is associated with rounder faces with wider eyebrows and more prominent jaws (Windhager et al. 2011). While these studies demonstrate that there are specific facial morphological features associated with formidability-related traits and that people pay attention to these traits, they do not demonstrate whether these particular features are indeed present in more formidable individuals. Using the geometric morphometric methods, Třebický et al. (2013) showed systematic differences in structural configurations of facial features, perceived aggressiveness, and actual fighting success in sample of professional fighters (although, the association between facial configuration and fighting success was restricted to heavyweight fighters). Shape regressions revealed that aggressive-looking faces are generally wider and have broader chins, more prominent eyebrows, and larger noses than less aggressive-looking faces.

Facial Width-to-Height Ratio (fWHR)

Currently, the most frequently investigated facial measure is the facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR or relative facial width) (Weston et al. 2007). Previous studies found that fWHR is related to several behavioral and personality characteristics including aggressive behavior and perceived aggressiveness (Geniole and McCormick 2015; Haselhuhn et al. 2015), even in cross-cultural context (Short et al. 2012). Stirrat et al. (2012) found that narrow-faced men were more likely than wider-faced men to die by contact violence compared to other causes of death or homicide. In a sample of professional MMA fighters, the variation in fWHR is associated with actual fighting performance, perception of aggressiveness, fighting ability, body weight (but not height) (Třebický et al. 2015), and higher numbers and proportions of wins and longer fighting careers (Zilioli et al. 2014).

Future Directions

One, currently rather neglected aspect of formidability related research, is cohesiveness of coalitions that seems to influence the outcome of conflicts. Most of the studies conducted so far are aimed on only one specific scenario of physical confrontations, where only two opponents confront. Though, physical encounters among men are not limited solely to the two individual opponents and may frequently involve other, the allies. Of high adaptive importance would be the ability to assess the prowess of a potential opponent as that of a potential ally, in terms of deciding whether to enter the fight and pursue relevant joined activities. Assessments of formidability are therefore not relevant solely in the context of a potential opponent but also in the context of selection of allies. Some species, particularly social primates, assess alliances and cooperative value and show deference toward individuals with more allies or with the ability to deny benefits (Smuts et al. 1987). Men with higher fWHR also show some evidence of increased sensitivity to coalitional alliance (Stirrat and Perrett 2012).

Many factors were suggested to affect assessments of formidability; nevertheless, it is not yet fully understood how these assessments work in real-life situations. We propose a model of multi-level assessments of formidability that tries to cover processes responsible for the fight-or-flight decision when faced with a potential antagonist. In terms of decision-making models, this would correspond to “The fast and frugal tree” model where an individual follows a decision-tree with an exit option after each attribute (Martignon et al. 2003). We suggest that assessment of potential opponents acts on multiple levels. Based on the knowledge of one’s own formidability, a resource holding power (Parker 1974) of each competitor is evaluated regarding the potential costs he can inflict and benefits he can gain from confrontation, so that the welfare trade-off ratios (Tooby et al. 2008) which help to recalibrate appropriate level of anger of rivals could be calculated (Sell 2011). In general, judgments of fighting ability seem to focus on overall probability of winning a fight. The first step, in such a multilevel fight-or-flight decision-making process, might depend predominantly on the overall size of the opponent, as suggested by the ratings of fighting ability in our previous study (Třebický et al. 2015). If one of the competitors is substantially larger (e.g., taller, more muscular, or bulkier), the smaller one surrenders and withdraws from the conflict. When the rivals have roughly equal sizes, a further level of assessment takes place – which might be related to the perception of bargaining potential such as the assessment of aggressiveness from facial features (Třebický et al. 2013). If both competitors feel equally formidable, the confrontation may get escalated.

Conclusion

Antagonistic conflicts with conspecifics are a fundamental factor influencing fitness in many social species, including humans. Fighting ability had profound implications in our evolutionary past and, in some communities, continues to have significant implications in contemporary societies. Evolution of adaptations that facilitate decision-making in potentially agonistic interactions is therefore expected, as the individual must determine whether is it best to fight, flee, or appease the prospective foe. According to the recalibration theory of anger (Sell 2011), benefits of minimizing the costs of engaging in violent conflict are thought to have shaped adaptations for the assessment of others’ capacity to cause physical harm.

The main aim of this chapter was to review evidence whether morphological features served as reliable cues to the actual formidability and whether our visual perception is sensitive towards such cues. Current body of evidence support this notion and show that morphological features predicts which of two opponents are likely to withdraw from an aggressive encounter and based upon converging results of current studies investigating competition in men, we can conclude that our morphological features are valid predictive cues of actual performance in physical fights and that raters are sensitive toward these cues. These studies demonstrate that there are specific morphological features associated with formidability-related traits and that people pay attention to these traits. Raters are able to distinguish the winner of consequent fights with accuracy higher than chance (Little et al. 2015); the winners are rated as better fighters, as stronger, more dominant and attractive, compared to losers (Little et al. 2015). Holistic facial shape analyses and simple facial features ratios showed systematic differences in structural configurations of facial morphology in successful competitors, compared to unsuccessful ones and between individuals perceived as more aggressive compared to less-aggressive ones (Třebický et al. 2013, 2015).

At first glance, it seems obvious that winning and losing in a physical fight is determined by physical strength and body size. Height and bulk are two physical characteristics that are easily observable traits that can be used to provide a relatively crude, albeit somewhat accurate, appraisal of fighting ability among adolescent males (Beaver et al. 2015). Although assortment of other factors like health, skill, hand–eye coordination, proper technique, and armaments may come at play.

Currently, the research mainly focused on thin slices of information provided by a single cue rated on a single behavioral/personality characteristic. However, our perception is not limited to gathering information through just one perceptual modality, e.g., just with our sight and we do ascribe many characteristics based on a single slice of information. Utilizing multiple cues assessment and/or multiple characteristics rating, testing the agreement between modalities and scales, and their subsequent interplay remains a challenge for future studies.

We propose that the assessment of potential opponents acts on multiple levels, then simply based on first impressions. In this multilevel “fight or flight” decision-making model, individuals assess their potential competitors following a decision-tree with an exit option after each attribute. At the first level, the decision-making process might depend predominantly on the overall size comparisons.

Although, competition in humans becomes widely investigated area in behavioral sciences, we are still at realms of rather skin-deep understanding to how the assessments of potential opponents take place. Abovementioned evidence is an important contribution to emerging body of research concerning evolution of neurocognitive and behavioral processes fine-tuned toward perception and adequate behavioral responses toward potential opponents.

Cross-References

Notes

Acknowledgement

VT and JH are supported by the Czech Science Foundation GAČR P407/16/03899S, by Charles University Research Centre program No. 204056, and by the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports NPU I program No. LO1611.

References

  1. Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology, 8(4), 291–322.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(3–4), 249–266; discussion 266–311.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09990951.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Arnott, G. (2017). Evolution of fighting assessment abilities. In T. K. Shackelford & V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Cham: Springer International Publishing.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6.Google Scholar
  4. Arnott, G., & Elwood, R. W. (2009). Assessment of fighting ability in animal contests. Animal Behaviour, 77(5), 991–1004.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.02.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beaver, K. M., Connolly, E. J., & Schwartz, J. A. (2015). Male physical fighting ability during adolescence is influenced by height and bulk. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, 1(4), 434–446.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40865-015-0020-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bergman, T. J., Ho, L., & Beehner, J. C. (2009). Chest color and social status in male geladas (Theropithecus gelada). International Journal of Primatology, 30(6), 791–806.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-009-9374-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Berry, D. S. (1991). Accuracy in social perception: Contributions of facial and vocal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 298–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brink, O., Vesterby, A., & Jensen, J. (1998). Pattern of injuries due to interpersonal violence. Injury, 29(9), 705–709.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0020-1383(98)00176-4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Burton, C. M., & Rule, N. O. (2013). Target effects judgments of height from faces are informedby dominance and facial maturity. Social Cognition, 31(6), 672–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Carré, J. M., McCormick, C. M., & Mondloch, C. J. (2009). Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1194–1198.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Carré, J. M., Murphy, K. R., & Hariri, A. R. (2013). What lies beneath the face of aggression? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(2), 224–229.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr096.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Carrier, D. R., & Morgan, M. H. (2014). Protective buttressing of the hominin face. Biological Reviews, 90(1), 330–346.  https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Coetzee, V., Chen, J., Perrett, D. I., & Stephen, I. D. (2010). Deciphering faces: Quantifiable visual cues to weight. Perception, 39(1), 51–61.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p6560.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dixson, B. J. W., Sherlock, J. M., Cornwell, W. K., & Kasumovic, M. M. (2017). Contest competition and men’s facial hair: Beards may not provide advantages in combat. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39, 147.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Doll, L. M., Hill, A. K., Rotella, M. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Welling, L. L. M., Wheatley, J. R., & Puts, D. A. (2014). How well do men’s faces and voices index mate quality and dominance? Human Nature, 25(2), 200–212.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9194-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Duntley, J. D. (2005). Adaptations to dangers from humans. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 224–249). Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  17. Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(4), 257–333.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(95)00050-U.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Enlow, D. H., Hans, M. H. G., & McGrew, L. (1996). In Saunders (Ed.), Essentials of facial growth. Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
  19. Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19(1), 82–87.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20583.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Fretwell, S. (1969). Dominance behavior and winter habitat distribution in juncos (Junco hyemalis). Bird-Banding, 40(1), 1.  https://doi.org/10.2307/4511533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Frýdlová, P., Šimková, O., Janovská, V., Velenský, P., & Frynta, D. (2017). Offenders tend to be heavier: Experimental encounters in mangrove-dwelling monitor lizards (Varanus indicus). Acta Ethologica, 20(1), 37–45.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-016-0246-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Geniole, S. N., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Facing our ancestors: Judgements of aggression are consistent and related to the facial width-to-height ratio in men irrespective of beards. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 279–285.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.12.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ghazanfar, A. A., & Santos, L. R. (2004). Primate brains in the wild: The sensory bases for social interactions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(8), 603–616.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1473.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Green, T. A. (2010). Martial arts of the world: An encyclopedia of history and innovation (Vol. 2). Santa Barbara: ABC Clio.Google Scholar
  25. Han, C., Kandrik, M., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Feinberg, D. R., Holzleitner, I. J., & …, Jones, B. C. (2017). Interrelationships among men’s threat potential, facial dominance, and vocal dominance. Evolutionary Psychology, 15(1), 147470491769733.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704917697332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haselhuhn, M. P., Ormiston, M. E., & Wong, E. M. (2015). Men’s facial width-to-height ratio predicts aggression: A meta-analysis. PLoS One, 10(4), e0122637.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122637.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Hill, A. K., Hunt, J., Welling, L. L. M. M., Cárdenas, R. A., Rotella, M. A., Wheatley, J. R., et al. (2013). Quantifying the strength and form of sexual selection on men’s traits. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(5), 334–341.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.05.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Holzleitner, I. J., & Perrett, D. I. (2016). Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(3), 217–229.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Horns, J., Jung, R., & Carrier, D. R. (2015). In vitro strain in human metacarpal bones during striking: Testing the pugilism hypothesis of hominin hand evolution. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218(20), 3215–3221.  https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.125831.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hugill, N., Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2009). Men’s physical strength is associated with women’s perceptions of their dancing ability. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(5), 527–530.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jeanmonod, R. K., Jeanmonod, D., Damewood, S., Perry, C., Powers, M., & Lazansky, V. (2011). Punch injuries: Insights into intentional closed fist injuries. The Western Journal of Emergency Medicine, 12(1), 6–10.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajem.2012.11.024.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. King, R. (2013). Fists of furry: At what point did human fists part company with the rest of the hominid lineage? Journal of Experimental Biology, 216(12), 2361–2361.  https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.085597.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Kraus, M. W., & Chen, T.-W. D. (2013). A winning smile? Smile intensity, physical dominance, and fighter performance. Emotion, 13(2), 270–279.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030745.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lassek, W. D., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (2009). Costs and benefits of fat-free muscle mass in men: Relationship to mating success, dietary requirements, and native immunity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(5), 322–328.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.04.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Little, A. C., Třebický, V., Havlíček, J., Roberts, S. C., & Kleisner, K. (2015). Human perception of fighting ability: facial cues predict winners and losers in mixed martial arts fights. Behavioral Ecology, 26(6), 1470–1475.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Manson, J. H., Wrangham, R. W., Boone, J. L., Chapais, B., Dunbar, R. I. M., Ember, C. R., et al. (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Current Anthropology, 32(4), 369–390.  https://doi.org/10.1086/203974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Martignon, L., Vitouch, O., Takezawa, M., & Forster, M. R. (2003). Naive and yet enlightened: From natural frequencies to fast and frugal decision trees. In Thinking: Psychological perspective on reasoning, judgment, and decision making (pp. 189–211). Chichester: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Morgan, M. H., & Carrier, D. R. (2013). Protective buttressing of the human fist and the evolution of hominin hands. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 216(Pt 2), 236–244.  https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.075713.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Neil, S. J. (1985). Size assessment and cues: Studies of hermit crab contests. Behaviour, 92(1/2), 22–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Palmer-Hague, J. L., Zilioli, S., Jagore, J., & DeLecce, T. L. (2015). Body mass index predicts fighting ability in female UFC fighters, but facial width-to-height ratio may not. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology., 2, 185.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-015-0035-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Parker, G. A. (1974). Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47(1), 223–243.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-5193(74)90111-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Pollet, T. V., Stulp, G., & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2013). Born to win? Testing the fighting hypothesis in realistic fights: Left-handedness in the Ultimate Fighting Championship. Animal Behaviour, 86(4), 839–843.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Price, M. E., Dunn, J., Hopkins, S., & Kang, J. (2012). Anthropometric correlates of human anger. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(3), 174–181.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.08.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: Mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 157–175.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Re, D. E., Debruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2013a). Facial cues to perceived height influence leadership choices in simulated war and peace contexts. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(1), 89–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Re, D. E., Hunter, D. W., Coetzee, V., Tiddeman, B. P., Xiao, D., Debruine, L. M., & …, Perrett, D. I. (2013b). Looking like a leader-facial shape predicts perceived height and leadership ability. PLoS One, 8(12), e80957.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Roper, M. K. (1969). A survey of the evidence for intrahuman killing in the Pleistocene. Current Anthropology, 10(4), 427–459.  https://doi.org/10.1086/201038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rushen, J. (1987). A difference in weight reduces fighting when unacquaintednewly weande pigs first meet. Canadian Journal of Animal Science, 67(4), 951–960.  https://doi.org/10.4141/cjas87-100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Scoggin, J. F., Brusovanik, G., Pi, M., Izuka, B., Pang, P., Tokumura, S., & Scuderi, G. (2010). Assessment of injuries sustained in mixed martial arts competition. American Journal of Orthopedics, 39(5), 247–251. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20567743.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Sell, A. N. (2011). The recalibrational theory and violent anger. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(5), 381–389.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2011.04.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sell, A. N. (2017). Recalibration theory of anger. In T. K. Shackelford & V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science (pp. 1–3). Cham: Springer International Publishing.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sell, A. N., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009a). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1656), 575–584.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1177.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Sell, A. N., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009b). Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(35), 15073–15078.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0904312106.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Sell, A., Bryant, G. A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & …, Gurven, M. (2010). Adaptations in humans for assessing physical strength from the voice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1699), 3509–3518.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sell, A. N., Hone, L. S. E., & Pound, N. (2012). The importance of physical strength to human males. Human Nature, 23(1), 30–44.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-012-9131-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Sell, A. N., Lukazsweski, A. W., & Townsley, M. (2017). Cues of upper body strength account for most of the variance in men’s bodily attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1869), 20171819.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1819.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Senar, J. C., & Camerino, M. (1998). Status signalling and the ability to recognize dominants: An experiment with siskins (Carduelis spinus). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1405), 1515–1520.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Shepherd, J. P., Shapland, M., Pearce, N. X., & Scully, C. (1990). Pattern, severity and aetiology of injuries in victims of assault. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 83(2), 75–78.  https://doi.org/10.1177/014107689008300206.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Short, L. A., Mondloch, C. J., McCormick, C. M., Carré, J. M., Ma, R., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2012). Detection of propensity for aggression based on facial structure irrespective of face race. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(2), 121–129.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.07.002.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Smuts, B., Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., Struhsaker, T., & Wrangham, R. (1987). Primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  61. Stillman, T. F., Maner, J. K., & Baumeister, R. F. (2010). A thin slice of violence: Distinguishing violent from nonviolent sex offenders at a glance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(4), 298–303.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.12.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Stirrat, M. R., & Perrett, D. I. (2012). Face structure predicts cooperation: Men with wider faces are more generous to their in-group when out-group competition is salient. Psychological Science, 23(7), 718–722.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611435133.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Stirrat, M. R., Stulp, G., & Pollet, T. V. (2012). Male facial width is associated with death by contact violence: Narrow-faced males are more likely to die from contact violence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 551–556.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Tibbetts, E. A., & Lindsay, R. (2008). Visual signals of status and rival assessment in Polistes dominulus paper wasps. Biology Letters, 4(3), 237–239.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0048.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., Sell, A. N., Lieberman, D., & Sznycer, D. (2008). Internal regulatory variables and the design of human motivation: A computational and evolutionary approach. In A. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.  https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203888148.ch15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Toscano, H., Schubert, T., & Sell, A. N. (2014). Judgments of dominance from the face track physical strength. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(1), 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Třebický, V., & Havlíček, J. (2017). Signals of body size. In T. K. Shackelford & V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Cham: Springer International Publishing.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Třebický, V., Kleisner, K., & Havlíček, J. (2012). Evolutionary concepts of human physical attractiveness: The case of male physique. Anthropologie, 50(1), 33–45.Google Scholar
  69. Třebický, V., Havlíček, J., Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., & Kleisner, K. (2013). Perceived aggressiveness predicts fighting performance in mixed-martial-arts fighters. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1664–1672.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613477117.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Třebický, V., Fialová, J., Kleisner, K., Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., & Havlíček, J. (2015). Further evidence for links between facial width-to-height ratio and fighting success: Commentary on Zilioli et al. (2014). Aggressive Behavior, 41(4), 331–334.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21559.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Walker, P. L. (1997). Wife beating, boxing, and broken noses: Skeletal evidence for the cultural patterning of interpersonal violence. In D. Frayer & D. Martin (Eds.), In troubled times: Violence and warfare in the past. London: Gordon and Breach.Google Scholar
  72. Walker, P. L. (2001). A bioarchaeological perspective on the history of violence. Annual Review of Anthropology, 30, 573–596. https://doi.org/0084-6570/01/1021-0573$14.00.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Watkins, C. D., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Priming men with different contest outcomes modulates their dominance perceptions. Behavioral Ecology, 23(3), 539–543.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Watkins, C. D., Fraccaro, P. J., Smith, F. G., Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., Debruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2010a). Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men. Behavioral Ecology, 21(5), 943–947.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Watkins, C. D., Jones, B. C., & Debruine, L. M. (2010b). Individual differences in dominance perception: Dominant men are less sensitive to facial cues of male dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 967–971.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Weston, E. M., Friday, A. E., & Lio, P. (2007). Biometric evidence that sexual selection has shaped the hominin face. PLoS One, 2(8), e710.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23(6), 805–814.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.21219.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Wu, X.-J., Schepartz, L. A., Liu, W., & Trinkaus, E. (2011). Antemortem trauma and survival in the late Middle Pleistocene human cranium from Maba, South China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(49), 19558–19562.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117113108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Zilioli, S., Sell, A. N., Stirrat, M. R., Jagor, J., Vickerman, W., Watson, N. V., & Jagore, J. (2014). Face of a fighter: Bizygomatic width as a cue of formidability. Aggressive Behavior, 41(4), 322–330.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Vít Třebický
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Michael Stirrat
    • 3
  • Jan Havlíček
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.National Institute of Mental HealthKlecanyCzech Republic
  2. 2.Faculty of ScienceCharles UniversityPragueCzech Republic
  3. 3.School of Psychological & Social SciencesYork St John UniversityYorkUK

Section editors and affiliations

  • Russell Jackson
    • 1
  1. 1.University of IdahoMoscowUSA