Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Vigilance, Sentinels, and Alarms

  • Fernando A. CamposEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2704-1

Synonyms

Definition

Vigilance is attentive behavior directed at an animal’s surroundings to detect or monitor perceived dangers. Sentinels are individuals in a social group that scan the surroundings while groupmates are engaged in other activities nearby. Alarm calls are vocalizations given by animals in response to perceived danger from predators.

Introduction

Predation is a fundamentally important selective pressure.

Given the large fitness costs of failing to detect and avoid predators, there should be strong selective pressure for behaviors that enable the early detection of possible threats.

Vigilance

Vigilance involves scanning the environment for possible dangers. Despite its clear individual benefits, vigilance may have significant opportunity costs in the form of time taken away from other important activities, such as foraging, mating, and socializing. Accordingly, vigilance may have chronic effects on individual fitness. Thus, if predation risk varies...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bednekoff, P. A. (2015). Chapter four – Sentinel behavior: A review and prospectus. In H. Jane Brockmann, M. Naguib, J. C. Mitani, L. W. Simmons, L. Barrett, S. Healy, & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 47, pp. 115–145). Amsterdam: Academic. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065345415000030.Google Scholar
  2. Clutton-Brock, T. H., O’Riain, M. J., Brotherton, P. N. M., Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., Griffin, A. S., & Manser, M. (1999). Selfish sentinels in cooperative mammals. Science, 284(5420), 1640–1644. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.284.5420.1640.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Lima, S. L., & Dill, L. M. (1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68(4), 619–640. https://doi.org/10.1139/z90-092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Marler, P., Evans, C., & Hauser, M. (1992). Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both? In H. Papousek, U. Jürgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 66–86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Roberts, G. (1996). Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. Animal Behaviour, 51(5), 1077–1086. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210(4471), 801–803. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7433999.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Sherman, P. W. (1977). Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science, 197(4310), 1246–1253. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.197.4310.1246.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Smith, J. M. (1965). The evolution of alarm calls. The American Naturalist, 99(904), 59–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Smythe, N. (1970). On the existence of “pursuit invitation” signals in mammals. The American Naturalist, 104(939), 491–494. https://doi.org/10.1086/282684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Treves, A. (2000). Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Animal Behaviour, 60(6), 711–722. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1528.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Tulane UniversityNew OrleansUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Russell Jackson
    • 1
  1. 1.University of IdahoMoscowUSA