Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Evolution of Emotion in Social Context

Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2459-1

Synonyms

Definition

Human emotion evolved – that is, came to be via natural selection – in an inherently social context; human emotions are therefore not separable from their social nature.

Introduction

While it is widely recognized that there is an important interplay between emotions and human social life, the social mechanisms by which emotions came to be via natural selection are not often considered. Extant theorizing on the evolution of emotions (hereafter referred to as EvoEmo) has traditionally focused on the modular cognitive or neural architecture that produces a small set of emotions, which are believed to serve discrete functions in the service of reproductive fitness (Cosmides and Tooby 2000; Tooby and Cosmides 2008). In these modular views, social functions are either not addressed or are just one type of function that may be important for reproductive success. Importantly, however, such modular theories are not the...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Abbott, L. F., & Kandel, E. R. (2012). A computational approach enhances learning in Aplysia. Nature Neuroscience, 15(2), 178–179.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3030.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Allada, R., & Chung, B. Y. (2010). Circadian organization of behavior and physiology in Drosophila. Annual Review of Physiology, 72(1), 605–624.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135815.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Almécija, S., Smaers, J. B., & Jungers, W. L. (2015). The evolution of human and ape hand proportions. Nature Communications, 6(1), 7717.  https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8717.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, D. J., & Adolphs, R. (2014). A framework for studying emotions across phylogeny. Cell, 157(1), 187–200.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Aquino, K., McFerran, B., & Laven, M. (2011). Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon goodness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 703–718.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022540.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Barrett, L. F. (2011). Constructing emotion. Psychological Topics, 20(3), 359–380. http://pt.ffri.hr/index.php/pt/article/view/45.Google Scholar
  7. Barrett, L. F. (2013). Psychological construction: The Darwinian approach to the science of emotion. Emotion Review, 5(4), 379–389.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barrett, L. F. (2017a). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
  9. Barrett, L. F. (2017b). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–23.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx060.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 167–218.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00404-8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3950.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Barrett, L. F., Khan, Z., Dy, J., & Brooks, D. (2018). Nature of emotion categories: Comment on Cowen and Keltner. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 97.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.12.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 17(4), 319–325.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Bartlett, M. Y., Condon, P., Cruz, J., Baumann, J., & Desteno, D. (2012). Gratitude: Prompting behaviours that build relationships. Cognition & emotion, 26(1), 2–13.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.561297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Beckes, L., & Coan, J. A. (2011). Our social baseline: The role of social proximity in economy of action. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(12), 976–988.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00400.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bell, W. J., & Sams, G. R. (1973). Aggressiveness in the cockroach Periplaneta americana (Orthoptera, Blattidae). Behavioral Biology, 9(5), 581–593.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-6773(73)80052-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Berscheid, E. (2003). The human’s greatest strength: Other humans. In A psychology of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psychology (pp. 37–47). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.  https://doi.org/10.1037/10566-003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Bliss-Moreau, E. (2017). Constructing nonhuman animal emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 184–188.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copysyc.2017.07.011.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Bliss-Moreau, E., Machado, C. J., & Amaral, D. G. (2013). Macaque cardiac physiology is sensitive to the valence of passively viewed sensory stimuli. PLoS One, 8(8), e71170.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071170.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2012). The construction of emotion in interactions, relationships, and cultures. Emotion Review, 4(3), 221–229.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912439765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Buss, D. M. (2018). Sexual and emotional infidelity: Evolved gender differences in jealousy prove robust and replicable. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 155–160.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617698225.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Cacioppo, J. T., Bernston, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  23. Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Cole, S. W., Capitanio, J. P., Goossens, L., & Boomsma, D. I. (2015). Loneliness across phylogeny and a call for comparative studies and animal models. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 202–212.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614564876.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Caldji, C., Tannenbaum, B., Sharma, S., Francis, D., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(9), 5335–5340.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Capitanio, J. P., Hawkley, L. C., Cole, S. W., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). A behavioral taxonomy of loneliness in humans and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). PLoS One, 9(10), e110307.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110307.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Chanes, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Redefining the role of limbic areas in cortical processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 96.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.11.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Chang, Y. P., Lin, Y. C., & Chen, L. H. (2012). Pay it forward: Gratitude in social networks. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(5), 761–781.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-011-9289-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Christensson, K., Siles, C., Moreno, L., Belaustequi, A., De la Fuente, P., Lagercrantz, H., … Winberg, J. (1992). Temperature, metabolic adaptation and crying in healthy full-term newborn infants cared for skin-to-skin or in a cot. Acta Paediatrica, 81, 488–493.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.1992.tb12280.x.
  29. Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2013). Psychological construction in the OCC model of emotion. Emotion Review, 5(4), 335–343.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489751.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1032–1039.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants’ reactions to simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54(1), 185–193.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1129876.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Comer, C. M. (1985). Analyzing cockroach escape behavior with lesions of individual giant interneurons. Brain Research, 335(2), 342–346.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(85)90490-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Cong, Y.-Q., Junge, C., Aktar, E., Raijmakers, M., Franklin, A., & Sauter, D. (2018). Pre-verbal infants perceive emotional facial expressions categorically. Cognition and Emotion, 1–13.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1455640.
  34. Cordaro, D. T., Sun, R., Keltner, D., Kamble, S., Huddar, N., & McNeil, G. (2018). Universals and cultural variations in 22 emotional expressions across five cultures. Emotion, 18(1), 75–93.  https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000302.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (Vol. 2, 2nd ed., pp. 91–115). NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  36. Craig, A. D. B. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59–70.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Craig, A. D. B. (2011). Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1225(1), 72–82.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Crespi, B. J. (2001). The evolution of social behavior in microorganisms. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 16(4), 178–183.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02115-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Critchley, H. D., & Nagai, Y. (2012). How emotions are shaped by bodily states. Emotion Review, 4(2), 163–168.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911430132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A., & Stillman, P. E. (2013). Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emotion Review, 5(4), 344–355.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Dankert, H., Wang, L., Hoopfer, E. D., Anderson, D. J., & Perona, P. (2009). Automated monitoring and analysis of social behavior in Drosophila. Nature Methods, 6(4), 297–303.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1310.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition & Emotion, 21(5), 1025–1042.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600980874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. DeSteno, D., Bartlett, M. Y., Baumann, J., Williams, L. A., & Dickens, L. (2010). Gratitude as moral sentiment: Emotion-guided cooperation in economic exchange. Emotion, 10(2), 289–293.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017883.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Doi, N., & Toh, Y. (1992). Modification of cockroach behavior to environmental humidity change by dehydration (Dictyoptera: Blattidae). Journal of Insect Behavior, 5(4), 479–490.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01058193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Pickering, T. R., Almécija, S., Heaton, J. L., Baquedano, E., Mabulla, A., & Uribelarrea, D. (2015). Earliest modern human-like hand bone from a new >1.84- million-year-old site at Olduvai in Tanzania. Nature Communications, 6(1), 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3(4), 364–370.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911410740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Fang, X., Sauter, D. A., & van Kleef, G. A. (2018). Seeing mixed emotions: The specificity of emotion perception from static and dynamic facial expressions across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49(1), 130–148.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117736270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Keltner, D. J. (2012). Flustered and faithful: Embarrassment as a signal of prosociality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 81–97.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025403.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C., & Suomi, S. J. (2009). Reciprocal face-to-face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and their newborn infants. Current Biology, 19(20), 1768–1772.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.055.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Field, T. M. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behavior & Development, 7(4), 517–522.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80010-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Field, T. M., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Bendell, D., Schanberg, S., … Kuhn, C. (1988). Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Development, 59(6), 1569.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130671.
  52. Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2008). Social functions of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  53. Gendron, M., & Barrett, L. F. (2009). Reconstructing the past: A century of ideas about emotion in psychology. Emotion Review, 1(4), 316–339.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073909338877.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Gendron, M., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotion perception as conceptual synchrony. Emotion Review, 10(2), 101–110.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073917705717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Gendron, M., Roberson, D., van der Vyver, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2014a). Cultural relativity in perceiving emotion from vocalizations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 911–920.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613517239.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Gendron, M., Roberson, D., Van Der Vyver, J. M., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2014b). Perceptions of emotion from facial expressions are not culturally universal: Evidence from a remote culture. Emotion, 14(2), 251–262.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036052.Perceptions.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Gendron, M., Roberson, D., & Barrett, L. F. (2015). Cultural variation in emotion perception is real: A response to Sauter, Eisner, Ekman, and Scott (2015). Psychological Science, 26(3), 357–359.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614566659.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2016). Other minds: The octopus, the sea, and the deep origins of consciousness. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. https://doi.org/DDC: 612.8.Google Scholar
  59. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2017). The evolution of consciousness in phylogenetic context. In K. Andrews & J. Beck (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of animal minds (pp. 1–20). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  60. Guillory, S. A., & Bujarski, K. A. (2014). Exploring emotions using invasive methods: Review of 60 years of human intracranial electrophysiology. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 1880–1889.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu002.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1965). The effect of rearing condition on behavior. International Journal of Psychiatry, 1, 43–51.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Harlow, H. F., & Suomi, S. J. (1970). Nature of love – Simplified. American Psychologist, 25(2), 161–168.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029383.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Harlow, H. F., & Zimmermann, R. R. (1959). Affectional response in the infant monkey: Orphaned baby monkeys develop a strong and persistent attachment to inanimate surrogate mothers. Science, 130(3373), 421–432.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.130.3373.421.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Helm, J. L., Sbarra, D. A., & Ferrer, E. (2014). Coregulation of respiratory sinus arrhythmia in adult romantic partners. Emotion, 14(3), 522–531.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035960.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Hofer, M. A. (1975). Studies on how early maternal separation produces behavioral change in young rats. Psychosomatic Medicine, 37(3), 245–264.  https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-197505000-00003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 1–25.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163539.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Keltner, D. J., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 505–521.  https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17(3), 429–453.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000662.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 51(6), 706–716.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.x.Longitudinal.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Kim, J. J., & Thompson, R. E. (1997). Cerebellar circuits and synaptic mechanisms involved in classical eyeblink conditioning. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(4), 177–181.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10081-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Kober, H., Barrett, L. F., Joseph, J., Bliss-Moreau, E., Lindquist, K., & Wager, T. D. (2008). Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 42(2), 998–1031.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.059.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  73. Krause, J., & Ruxton, G. D. (2002). Living in groups. New York: Oxford University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1093/sysbio/sys022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394–421.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Lee, V., Cheal, J. L., & Rutherford, M. D. (2015). Categorical perception along the happy-angry and happy-sad continua in the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 40, 95–102.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. (2011). Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 834–855.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024244.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1983). Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 587–597.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.587.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Lindquist, K. A. (2013). Emotions emerge from more basic psychological ingredients: A modern psychological constructionist model. Emotion Review, 5(4), 356–368.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Lindquist, K. A. (2017). The role of language in emotion: Existing evidence and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 135–139.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COPSYC.2017.07.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000446.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Lindquist, K. A., Siegel, E. H., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). The hundred-year emotion war: Are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on Lench, Flores, and Bench (2011). Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 255–263.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029038.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  82. Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., & Gendron, M. (2015). Does language do more than communicate emotion? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 99–108.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414553440.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  83. Ma, L. K., Tunney, R. J., & Ferguson, E. (2017). Does gratitude enhance prosociality?: A meta- analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(6), 601.  https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000103.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Marzke, M. W. (2013). Tool making, hand morphology and fossil hominins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 368(1630), 20120414.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0414.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  85. Mesquita, B. (2010). Emoting: A contextualized process. In B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context (1st ed., pp. 83–104). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  86. Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2014). Emotions in context: A socio-dynamic model of emotions. Emotion Review.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914534480.
  87. Moroz, L. L. (2011). Aplysia. Current Biology, 21(2), R60–R61.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.028.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Universality revisited. Emotion Review, 5(1), 8–15.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912457227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., Dahl, R. E., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain – Behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 14(3), 499–520.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579402003061.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Niedenthal, P. M., & Brauer, M. (2012). Social functionality of human emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 259–285.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131605.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Nowak, M. A., & Roch, S. (2007). Upstream reciprocity and the evolution of gratitude. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274(1610), 605–610.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.0125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS One, 6(9), e21236.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021236.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  93. Papp, L. M., Pendry, P., Simon, C. D., & Adam, E. K. (2013). Spouses’ cortisol associations and moderators: Testing physiological synchrony and connectedness in everyday life. Family Process, 52(2), 284–298.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01413.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Parker, S. W., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). The impact of early institutional rearing on the ability to discriminate facial expressions of emotion: An event-related potential study. Child Development, 76(1), 54–72.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00829.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16(2), 331–348.  https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1087.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1(1), 60–85.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908097189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Ross, K. G., & Keller, L. (1998). Genetic control of social organization in an ant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(24), 14232–14237.  https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.95.24.14232.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  98. Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 102–141.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.102.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145–172.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(6), 2408–2412.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908239106.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  101. Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2015, March 21). Emotional vocalizations are recognized across cultures regardless of the valence of distractors. Psychological Science, 26, 354.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614560771.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  102. Saxbe, D., & Repetti, R. L. (2010). For better or worse? Coregulation of couples’ cortisol levels and mood states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(1), 92–103.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016959.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Schnall, S., Harber, K. D., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Social support and the perception of geographical slant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1246–1255.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.011.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  104. Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2011). What are emotion expressions for? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(6), 395–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Siegel, J. T., Thomson, A. L., & Navarro, M. A. (2014). Experimentally distinguishing elevation from gratitude: Oh, the morality. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9(5), 414–427.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.910825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., … Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 343–393.  https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000128.
  107. Spieth, H. T. (1974). Courtship behavior in Drosophila. Annual Review of Entomology, 19(1), 385–405.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.en.19.010174.002125.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Suomi, S. J., Collins, M. L., Harlow, H. F., & Ruppenthal, G. C. (1976). Effects of maternal and peer separations on young monkeys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(2), 101–112.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00382.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 114–137). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  111. Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., … Casey, B. J. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Developmental Science, 13(1), 46–61.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x.
  112. Tracy, J. L., & Randles, D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: A review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3(4), 397–405.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911410747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Tsang, J. A., & Martin, S. R. (2017). Four experiments on the relational dynamics and prosocial consequences of gratitude. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1–18.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2017.1388435.
  114. Van de Vyver, J., & Abrams, D. (2015). Testing the prosocial effectiveness of the prototypical moral emotions: Elevation increases benevolent behaviors and outrage increases justice behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 23–33.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.12.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. van Kleef, G. A. (2010). The emerging view of emotion as social information. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(5), 331–343.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00262.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. van Kleef, G. A., Cheshin, A., Fischer, A. H., & Schneider, I. K. (2016). Editorial: The social nature of emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 882.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. van Kleef, G. A., Heerdink, M. W., & Homan, A. C. (2017). Emotional influence in groups: The dynamic nexus of affect, cognition, and behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 156–161.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.017.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Wagner, H., & Lee, V. (1999). Facial behavior alone and in the presence of others. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 262–286). Cambridge, UK/Paris: Cambridge University Press/Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.Google Scholar
  119. Walters, E. T., Carew, T. J., & Kandel, E. R. (1981). Associative learning in Aplysia: Evidence for conditioned fear in an invertebrate. Science, 211(4481), 504–506.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455692.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Waters, S. F., & Mendes, W. B. (2016). Physiological and relational predictors of mother-infant behavioral coordination. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2(4), 298–310.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0045-9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  121. Waters, S. F., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2014). Stress contagion: Physiological covariation between mothers and infants. Psychological Science, 25(4), 934–942.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613518352.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  122. Watt Smitt, T. (2015). The book of human emotions. New York: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
  123. Webre, D. J., Wolanin, P. M., & Stock, J. B. (2003). Bacterial chemotaxis. Current Biology, 13(2), R47–R49.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01424-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Widen, S. C. (2013). Children’s interpretation of facial expressions: The long path from valence- based to specific discrete categories. Emotion Review, 5(1), 72–77.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures : Diversity and universals. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Williams, L. A., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2016). Humans are ultrasocial and emotional. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39(e117), 39–40.Google Scholar
  127. Williams, L. A., & DeSteno, D. (2014). Building bonds, attaining ambitions, and establishing esteem: How positive emotions serve intrapersonal needs. In J. Gruber & J. T. Moskowitz (Eds.), Positive emotion: Integrating the dark sides and the light sides (pp. 206–224). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1105–1127.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.032.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Wise, S. P. (2008). Forward frontal fields: Phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(12), 599–608.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.08.008.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  130. Wolanin, P. M., & Stock, J. B. (2004). Bacterial chemosensing: Cooperative molecular logic. Current Biology, 14(12), 486–487.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Young, R. W. (2003). Evolution of the human hand: The role of throwing and clubbing. Journal of Anatomy, 202(1), 165–174.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1469-7580.2003.00144.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, California National Primate Research CenterUniversity of CaliforniaDavisUSA
  2. 2.School of Psychology, University of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Section editors and affiliations

  • Carey Fitzgerald
    • 1
  1. 1.University of South Carolina – BeaufortBlufftonUSA