Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Evolution of Play

  • Christopher X Jon JensenEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1062-1

Synonyms

Definition

Play behavior can be studied as an adaptation evolved to develop motor and problem-solving skills, assess one’s own capabilities in relation to one’s environment, and learn how to interact and perhaps cooperate with social partners; play can also be considered phylogenetically as a trait that has evolved divergently, convergently, and in parallel.

Introduction

Play behavior is highly prevalent across much of the animal kingdom, especially in the mammalian lineages. This prevalence suggests that play behavior must be an evolved adaptation, but developing and testing hypotheses to explain its origins has proved challenging. Understanding play behavior from an evolutionary perspective requires that play be clearly defined, that we have clear hypotheses explaining the relative costs and benefits of play, and that these adaptive hypotheses can be tested through direct or indirect observation. This...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bateson, P. (2014). Play, playfulness, creativity and innovation. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 99–112.  https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.02.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bekoff, M., & Byers, J. A. (Eds.). (1998). Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bekoff, M., & Pierce, J. (2009). Wild justice: The moral lives of animals. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2015). Vygotskian and post-Vygotskian views on children’s play. American Journal of Play, 7(3), 371–388.Google Scholar
  5. Bradshaw, J. W. S., Pullen, A. J., & Rooney, N. J. (2015). Why do adult dogs ‘play’? Behavioural Processes, 110, 82–87.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.023.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown, S. (2009). Play: How it shapes the brain, opens the imagination, and invigorates the soul. New York: Avery.Google Scholar
  7. Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  8. Burghardt, G. M. (2014). A brief glimpse at the long evolutionary history of play. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 90–98.  https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.01.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cameron, E. Z., Linklater, W. L., Stafford, K. J., & Minot, E. O. (2008). Maternal investment results in better foal condition through increased play behaviour in horses. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1511–1518.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dallaire, J. A., & Mason, G. (2017). Juvenile rough-and-tumble play predicts adult sexual behaviour in American mink. Animal Behavior, 123, 81–89.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dallaire, J. A., Espinosa, J., & Mason, G. (2017). Play and optimal welfare: Does play indicate the presence of positive affective states? Behavioural Processes.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.011.
  12. Diamond, J., & Bond, A. B. (2003). A comparative analysis of social play in birds. Behaviour, 140, 1091–1115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dinets, V. (2015). Play behavior in crocodilians. Animal Behavior and Cognition 2(1):49–55. http://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.04.2015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fagen, R. M. (2017). Salmonid jumping and playing: Potential cultural and welfare implications. Animals, 7, 42.  https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7060042.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Fagen, R., & Fagen, J. (2004). Juvenile survival and benefits of play behaviour in brown bears, Ursus arctos. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 6, 89–102.Google Scholar
  16. Held, S. D. E., & Špinka, M. (2011). Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behavior, 81, 891–899.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Iwaniuk, A. N., Nelson, J. E., & Pellis, S. M. (2001). Do big-brained animals play more? Comparative analyses of play and relative brain size in mammals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(1), 29–41.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.29.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Kuba, M. J., Byrne, R. A., Meisel, D. V., & Mather, J. A. (2006). When do octopuses play? Effects of repeated testing, object type, age, and food deprivation on object play in Octopus vulgaris. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(3), 184–190.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.184.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Kuczaj, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). Why do dolphins play? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 113–127.  https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.03.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Landreth, G. L. (2002). Play therapy: The art of the relationship (2nd ed.). New York: Brunner-Routledge.Google Scholar
  21. Lee, P. C., & Moss, C. J. (2014). African elephant play, competence and social complexity. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 144–156.  https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.05.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Nowell, A. (2016). Childhood, play and the evolution of cultural capacity in Neanderthals and modern humans. In M. N. Haidle, N. J. Conard, & M. Bolus (Eds.), The nature of culture: Based on an interdisciplinary symposium ‘The Nature of Culture’, Tübingen, Germany. New York: Springer.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nunes, S., Muecke, E.-M., Sanchez, Z., Hoffmeir, R. R., & Lancaster, L. T. (2004). Play behavior and motor development in juvenile Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 97–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Osvath, M., Osvath, H., & Bååth, R. (2014). An exploration of play behaviors in raven nestlings. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 157–165.  https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.06.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Palagi, E. (2006). Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implications for natural social systems and interindividual relationships. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 129, 418–426.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20289.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Paulos, R. D., Trone, M., & Kuczaj, S. A., II. (2010). Play in wild and captive cetaceans. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(4), 701–722.Google Scholar
  27. Pellegrini, A. D. (2009). The role of play in human development. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pellis, S. M., & Iwaniuk, A. N. (1999). The roles of phylogeny and sociality in the evolution of social play in muroid rodents. Animal Behavior, 58, 361–373.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1999.1141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pruitt, J. N., Burghardt, G. M., & Riechert, S. E. (2012). Non-conceptive sexual behavior in spiders: A form of play associated with body condition, personality type, and male intrasexual selection. Ethology, 118, 33–40.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01980.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sommerville, R., O’Connor, E. A., & Asher, L. (2017). Why do dogs play? Function and welfare implications of play in the domestic dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 197, 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.09.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Spinka, M., Newberry, R. C., & Bekoff, M. (2001). Mammalian play: Training for the unexpected. Quarterly Review of Biology, 76(2), 141–168.  https://doi.org/10.1086/393866.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Mathematics & SciencePratt InstituteBrooklynUSA

Section editors and affiliations

  • Menelaos Apostolou
    • 1
  1. 1.University of NicosiaNicosiaCyprus