Advertisement

Study of Linguistic Areas: Evidence from Cultural Words, Semantic Maps, and Spatial Reference in Southeast Asia

  • Stefanie SiebenhütterEmail author
Reference work entry

Abstract

This chapter describes a case study of spatial reference in Thai, Lao, Khmer, and Vietnamese within the field of areal linguistics. As space is seen as one of the basic domains of conceptualization, spatial reference provides an exemplary approach for investigation at the conceptual-semantic level. It is widely agreed that Mainland Southeast Asia forms one of the prime examples of a linguistic area or Sprachbund. Traditionally, languages which show a significant amount of parallels on the morphological, phonetic, and structural level developed during long-term contacts even though they are belonging to different language families which form a distinct linguistic area. At this point, the conceptual-semantic level as a criterion for linguistic areas is underexplored in the literature. By identifying a significant number of parallels and conceptual borrowings of semantic value of static spatial reference, this chapter analyzes further opportunities for studying linguistic areas.

Keywords

Areal linguistics Conceptual transfer Semantic convergence Culture words Language contact ISO 639-3 codes: tha, lao, khm, vie, ind 

Notes

Publisher’s note:

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Bickel, B. (2007). Typology in the 21st century: Major current developments. Linguistic Typology, 11(1), 239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bowerman, M., & Pederson, E. (1992). Topological relations picture series. In S. C. Levinson (Ed.), Space stimuli kit 1.2. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
  3. Campbell, L. (2017). Why is it so hard to define a linguistic area? In R. Hickey (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of areal linguistics (Cambridge handbooks in language and linguistics, pp. 19–39). Cambridge, UK: University Printing House.Google Scholar
  4. Daller, M. H., Treffers-Daller, J., & Furman, R. (2011). Transfer of conceptualization patterns in bilinguals: The construal of motion events in Turkish and German. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(1), 95–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Danziger, E. (2010). Deixis, gesture, and cognition in spatial frame of reference typology. Studies in Language, 34(1), 167–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Ebert, K. (2001). Südasien als Sprachbund. In M. Haspelmath, E. König, W. Oesterreicher, & W. Raible (Eds.), Language typology and language universals. An international handbook = Sprachtypologie und sprachliche Universalien: ein internationales Handbuch (Handbücher zur Sprach- und Kommunikationswissenschaft/[HSK]; 20.2). Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  7. Enfield, N. J., & Comrie, B. (Eds.). (2015). Languages of Mainland Southeast Asia: The state of the art (von Pacific Linguistics [PL], Vol. 649). Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  8. Evans, V. (2007). How we conceptualise time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. In V. Evans, B. K. Bergen, & J. Zinken (Eds.), The cognitive linguistics reader: Advances in cognitive linguistics (pp. 733–765). London: Equinox Publishing.Google Scholar
  9. Evans, V. (Ed.). (2009). How words mean: Lexical concepts, cognitive models, and meaning construction (Oxford linguistics). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Fodor, J. (1998). Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. Oxford: Oxford Univeristy Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Frank, R. M. (2016). Cultural linguistics vis-à-vis cognitive linguistics: A critical perspective. A keynote speech at the First international conference of cultural linguistics, Prato, 20–22 July 2016.Google Scholar
  12. Gil, D. (2015). The Mekong-Mamberamo linguistic area. In N. J. Enfield & B. Comrie (Eds.), Languages of Mainland Southeast Asia: The state of the art (Pacific Linguistics series, Vol. 649, pp. 266–355). Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  13. Heine, B., & Kuteva, T. (2005). Language contact and grammatical change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I. (2012). The importance of unveiling conceptual metaphors in a minority language. In A. Idström & E. Piirainen (Eds.), Endangered metaphors (Cognitive linguistic studies in cultural contexts, pp. 253–274). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  15. Jaberg, K., & Jud, J. (1928). Der Sprachatlas als Forschungsinstrument. Kritische Grundlegung und Einführung in den Sprach- und Sachatlas Italiens und der Südschweiz. Halle (Saale): Niemeyer.Google Scholar
  16. Janda, L. A. (2006). From cognitive linguistics to cultural linguistics. In Slovo a smysl/Word and Sense, 8, 48–68.Google Scholar
  17. Jarvis, S. (2007). Theoretical and methodological issues in the investigation of conceptual transfer. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 4, 43–71.Google Scholar
  18. Jarvis, S. (2011). Conceptual transfer. Crosslinguistic effects in categorization and construal. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(1), 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jarvis, S. (2016). Clarifying the scope of conceptual transfer. Language Learning, 66(3), 608–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jarvis, S., & Pavlenko, A. (2008). Crosslinguistic influence in language and cognition. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lakoff, G. (2014). The all new don’t think of an elephant! Know your values and frame the debate. White River Junction: Chelsea Green Publishing.Google Scholar
  23. Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity (Language, culture, and cognition, Vol. 5). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Levinson, S. C., & Wilkins, D. P. (2006a). Patterns in the data: Towards a semantic typology of spatial description. In S. C. Levinson & D. P. Wilkins (Eds.), Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity (pp. 512–552). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Levinson, S. C., & Wilkins, D. P. (2006b). The background to the study of the language of space. In S. C. Levinson & D. P. Wilkins (Eds.), Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity (pp. 1–23). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Malt, B. C., Gennari, S., Imai, M., Ameel, E., Saji, N., & Majid, A. (2015). Where are the concepts? What words can and can’t reveal. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), The conceptual mind: New directions in the study of concepts (pp. 291–326). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  27. Odlin, T. (1989). Language transfer: Cross-linguistic influence in language learning (The Cambridge applied linguistics series). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Pavlenko, A., & Jarvis, S. (2002). Bidirectional transfer. Applied Linguistics, 23(2), 190–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. SEAlang Projects. (2013). SEAlang Library: Southeast Asian Languages Library. http://sealang.net. Accessed 2 June 2017.
  30. Sharifian, F. (2003). On cultural conceptualisations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3(3), 187–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Sharifian, F. (2011). Cultural conceptualisations and language: Theoretical framework and applications (Cognitive linguistic studies in cultural contexts series, #1). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  32. Siebenhütter, S. (2012). Topologische Relationen Im Vietnamesischen. Paper presented at the 51st Students’ Conference of Linguistics (StuTS), University of Stuttgart, 16–20 May 2012.Google Scholar
  33. Siebenhütter, S. (2016). Raum-Konzeptualisierungen im südostasiatischen ArealLaotisch, Thai, Khmer und Vietnamesisch. Dissertation, LMU Munich: Faculty for Language- and Literature Sciences.Google Scholar
  34. Stolz, T. (2002). No Sprachbund beyond this line! On the age-old discussion of how to define a linguistic area. In P. Ramat & T. Stolz (Eds.), Mediterranean languages. Papers from the MEDTYP workshop, Tirrenia, June 2000 (pp. 259–281). Bochum: Universitätsverlag.Google Scholar
  35. Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Volume 1: Language, speech, and communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Thiering, M. (2015). Spatial semiotics and spatial mental models: Figure-ground asymmetries in language (Applications of cognitive linguistics). Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
  37. Thomason, S. (2008). Social and linguistic factors as predictors of contact-induced change. Journal of Language Contact, 2(1), 42–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Thomason, S. G., & Kaufman, T. (1981, March). Are there linguistic prerequisites for contact-induced language change? Paper presented at the annual University of Wisconsin – Milwaukee Linguistics Symposium on Language Contact, Milwaukee.Google Scholar
  39. Tosco, M. (2008). What to do when you are unhappy with language areas but you do not want to quit. Journal of Language Contact – Thema, 2, 112–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Trubetzkoy, N. S. (1930). Proposition 16. Über den Sprachbund. In Actes du premier congrès international de linguistes à la Haye du 10–15 avril 1928 (pp. 17–18). Leiden: Sijthoff.Google Scholar
  41. Ungerer, F., & Schmid, H.-J. (1996). An introduction to cognitive linguistics (Learning about language, Vol. 1). London: Longman.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ludwig Maximilians University MunichMunichGermany
  2. 2.Department of English and LinguisticsJohannes Gutenberg University MainzMainzGermany

Personalised recommendations