Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders

Living Edition
| Editors: Fred R. Volkmar

Savant Syndrome

  • Meital GewirtzEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6435-8_102323-1

Definition

Savant syndrome is a disorder characterized by a niche area of immense talent that contrasts with a general intellectual impairment. Many people with savant syndrome also have autism. The condition is rare, but because of the striking skills that some savants have, it has captured the attention of the general public and researchers. Savant talents vary from exceptional mathematical abilities, to prodigious musical skills, to extraordinary artistic talent.

Historical Background

The first documented case of savant syndrome appeared in the literature in the late eighteenth century. In 1887, Dr. J. Langdon Down, well-known for his identification of Down’s syndrome, described savant syndrome as its own condition. In his lectures he described ten different cases that had been identified (Treffert 2014). Until several decades ago, savants were called “idiot savants.” This title is not only jarring to the modern ear, but entirely inaccurate. “Idiot” was formerly used to describe...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References and Readings

  1. Baio, J., Wiggins, L., Christensen, D. L., Maenner, M. J., Daniels, J., Warren, Z., … & Dowling, N. F. (2018). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years – Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 67(6), 1–23.  https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bennett, E. A., & Heaton, P. (2017). Defining the clinical and cognitive phenotype of child savants with autism spectrum disorder. Current Pediatric Research, 21(1), 140–147.Google Scholar
  3. Brink, T. L. (1980). Idiot savant with unusual mechanical ability: An organic explanation. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 137(2), 250–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brogaard, B. (2012). Kim Peek, the Real Rain Man. Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/the-superhuman-mind/201212/kim-peek-the-real-rain-man.
  5. Camulli, J. E., & Goh, L. A. (2018). Re-conceptualising autistic savantism as a spectrum syndromic disorder: A sequel to the case study of a young adult savant artist. European Journal of Special Education Research, 3(4), 185–204.Google Scholar
  6. Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2010). IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: Data from the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Psychological Medicine, 41(3), 619–627.  https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291710000991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Corrigan, N. M., Richards, T. L., Treffert, D. A., & Dager, S. R. (2012). Toward a better understanding of the savant brain. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53, 706–717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Crespi, B. J. (2016). Autism as a disorder of high intelligence. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10, 300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Höfer, P., & Röckenhaus, F. (2015). Beautiful minds. Retrieved from https://www.amazon.com/Beautiful-Minds-Petra-Höfer/dp/B015RRQR20. Accessed 27 Sept 2019.
  10. Hou, C., Miller, B. L., Cummings, J. L., Goldberg, M., Mychack, P., Bottino, V., & Benson, F. (2000). Artistic savants. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 13(1), 29–38.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Lythgoe, M. F., Pollak, T. A., Kalmus, M., Haan, M. D., & Chong, W. K. (2005). Obsessive, prolific artistic output following subarachnoid hemorrhage. Neurology, 64(2), 397–398.  https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000150526.09499.3e.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Ma, D., Jaworski, J., Menold, M., Donnelly, S., Abramson, R., Wright, H., … & Cuccaro, M. L. (2005). Ordered-subset analysis of savant skills in autism for 15q11-q13. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 135B(1), 38–41.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Moss, S. (2007). He attacked the keyboard. The Guardian.Google Scholar
  14. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., & Soulieres, I. (2009). Enhanced perception in savant syndrome: Patterns, structure and creativity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1385–1391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Nurmi, E. L., Dowd, M., Tadevosyan-Leyfer, O., Haines, J., Folstein, S. E., & Sutcliffe, J. S. (2003). Exploratory subsetting of autism families based on savant skills improves evidence of genetic linkage to 15q11-q13. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(7), 856–863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Rimland, B., & Hill, A. L. (1984). Idiot savants. In Mental retardation and developmental disabilities (pp. 155–169). Boston: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Smith, N. V., Tsimpli, I.-M., & Ouhalla, J. (1993). Learning the impossible: The acquisition of possible and impossible languages by a polyglot savant. Lingua, 91(4), 279–347.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(93)90002-e.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Treffert, D. A. (1988). The idiot savant: A review of the syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145(5), 563–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Treffert, D. A. (2009). The savant syndrome: An extraordinary condition. A synopsis: Past, present, future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1351–1357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Treffert, D. A. (2014). Savant syndrome: Realities, myths and misconceptions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 564–571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Yale UniversityNew HavenUSA