Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders

Living Edition
| Editors: Fred R. Volkmar

Double Empathy

  • Damian Elgin Maclean MiltonEmail author
  • Brett Heasman
  • Elizabeth Sheppard
Living reference work entry

Latest version View entry history

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6435-8_102273-2
  • 7 Downloads

Definition

The double empathy problem (DEP) refers to a “disjuncture in reciprocity between two differently disposed social actors” who hold different norms and expectations of each other, such as is common in autistic to non-autistic social interactions (Milton 2012, p. 884). With different dispositional outlooks and personal conceptual understandings, interactions involving autistic and non-autistic people are susceptible to frequent misunderstandings. It is a “double problem” as both people experience it, and so it is not a singular problem located in any one person. However, “the disjuncture may be more severe for the non-autistic disposition as it is experienced as unusual, while for the ‘autistic person’ it is a common experience” (Milton 2012, p. 885).

In principal, the DEP becomes more marked the wider the disjuncture in dispositional perceptions of what constitutes the social context. It is suggested that “social subtext is never fully given as a set of a priori...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References and Reading

  1. Beardon, L. (2017). Autism and Asperger syndrome in adults. London: Sheldon Press.Google Scholar
  2. Bolis, D., Balsters, J., Wenderoth, N., Becchio, C., & Shilbach, L. (2017). Introducing the dialectical misattunement hypothesis and a Bayesian account of Intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 50(6), 335–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brewer, R., Biotti, F., Catmur, C., Press, C., Happé, F., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2016). Can neurotypical individuals read autistic facial expressions? Atypical production of emotional facial expressions in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 9(2), 262–271.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Cage, E., Di Monaco, J., & Newell, V. (2018). Experiences of autism acceptance and mental health in autistic adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(2), 473–484.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3342-7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Chown, N. (2014). More on the ontological status of autism and double empathy. Disability & Society, 29(10), 1672–1676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Collins, H., & Evans, R. (2007). Rethinking expertise. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Crompton, C., Ropar, D., Evans-Williams, C., Flynn, E., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2019). Autistic peer to peer information transfer is highly effective.  https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/j4knx
  8. Dean, M., Harwood, R., & Kasari, C. (2017). The art of camouflage: Gender differences in the social behaviors of girls and boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(6), 678–689.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316671845.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Dewinter, J., Van Parys, H., Vermeiren, R., & Van Nieuwenhuizen, C. (2017). Adolescent boys with an autism spectrum disorder and their experience of sexuality: An interpretative phenomenological analysis. Autism, 21(1), 75–82.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315627134.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Edey, R., Cook, J., Brewer, R., Johnson, M. H., Bird, G., & Press, C. (2016). Interaction takes two: Typical adults exhibit mind-blindness towards those with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(7), 879–885.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Faso, D. J., Sasson, N. J., & Pinkham, A. E. (2015). Evaluating posed and evoked facial expressions of emotion from adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(1), 75–89.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Fletcher-Watson, S., Adams, J., Brook, K., Charman, T., Crane, L., Cusack, J., Leekham, S., Milton, D., Parr, J. R., & Pellicano, E. (2019). Making the future together: Shaping autism research through meaningful participation. Autism, 23(4), 943–953.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Garfinkel, H. (1964). Studies of the routine grounds of everyday activities. Social Problems, 11(3), 225–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gernsbacher, M. A., Stevenson, J. L., & Dern, S. (2017). Specificity, contexts, and reference groups matter when assessing autistic traits. PLoS One, 12(2), e0171931.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Goffman, E. (1958). The presentation of self in everyday life. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Grossman, R. B., Edelson, L. R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2013). Emotional facial and vocal expressions during story retelling by children and adolescents with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(3), 1035–1044.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Hacking, I. (1995). The looping effects of human kinds. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Symposia of the Fyssen Foundation. Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 351–394). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524021.003.0012.Google Scholar
  18. Heasman, B. (2018). Enabling autistic sociality: Unrealised potentials in two-sided interaction. Autism. PhD thesis, The London School of Economics and Political Science (LSE). http://etheses.lse.ac.uk/3864/
  19. Heasman, B., & Gillespie, A. (2018). Perspective-taking is two-sided: Misunderstandings between people with Asperger’s syndrome and their family members. Autism, 22(6), 740–750.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317708287.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Heasman, B., & Gillespie, A. (2019a). Neurodivergent intersubjectivity: Distinctive features of how autistic people create shared understanding. Autism, 23(4), 910–921.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318785172.
  21. Heasman, B., & Gillespie, A. (2019b). Participants over-estimate how helpful they are in a two-player game scenario toward an artificial confederate that discloses a diagnosis of autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(JUN), 1–15.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01349.
  22. Hendricks, D. (2010). Employment and adults with autism spectrum disorders: Challenges and strategies for success. Journal of Vocational Rehabilitation, 32(2), 125–134.  https://doi.org/10.3233/JVR-2010-0502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jaswal, V., & Akhtar, N. (2019). Being versus appearing socially uninterested: Challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, E82.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X18001826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kite, D. M., Gullifer, J., & Tyson, G. A. (2013). Views on the diagnostic labels of autism and Asperger’s disorder and the proposed changes in the DSM. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 1692–1700.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1718-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Macdonald, H., Rutter, M., Howlin, P., Rios, P., Conteur, A. L., Evered, C., & Folstein, S. (1989). Recognition and expression of emotional cues by autistic and normal adults. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(6), 865–877.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Maras, K. L., & Bowler, D. M. (2014). Eyewitness testimony in autism spectrum disorder: A review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(11), 2682–2697.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1502-3.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Mead, G. H. (1934). Thought, communication, and the significant symbol. In C. W. Morris (Ed.), Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist (pp. 68–75). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  28. Milton, D. (2012). On the ontological status of autism: The “double empathy problem.”. Disability & Society, 27(6), 883–887.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2012.710008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Milton, D. (2014a). Embodied sociality and the conditioned relativism of dispositional diversity. Autonomy, the Critical Journal of Interdisciplinary Autism Studies, 1(3), 1–7. http://www.larry-arnold.net/Autonomy/index.php/autonomy/article/view/AR10.Google Scholar
  30. Milton, D. (2014b). Autistic expertise: A critical reflection on the production of knowledge in autism studies. Autism, 18(7), 794–802.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314525281.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Milton, D. E. (2016a). Disposable dispositions: Reflections upon the work of Iris Marion young in relation to the social oppression of autistic people. Disability & Society, 31(10), 1403–1407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Milton, D. (2016b). Educational discourse and the autistic student: A study using Q-sort methodology. Birmingham: University of Birmingham.Google Scholar
  33. Milton, D., & Bracher, M. (2013). Autistics speak but are they heard. Journal of the BSA MedSoc Group, 7, 61–69.Google Scholar
  34. Milton, D., & Sims, T. (2016). How is a sense of well-being and belonging constructed in the accounts of autistic adults? Disability and Society, 31(4), 520–534.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2016.1186529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Moore, H., & Gillespie, A. (2014). The caregiving bind: Concealing the demands of informal care can undermine the caregiving identity. Social Science and Medicine, 116, 102–109.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.06.038.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Morrison, K. E., DeBrabander, K. M., Jones, D. R., Faso, D. J., Ackerman, R. A., & Sasson, N. J. (2019). Outcomes of real-world social interaction for autistic adults paired with autistic compared to typically developing partners. Autism.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319892701.
  37. Murray, D. (1992). Attention tunnelling and autism. In P. Shattock, & G. Linfoot (Eds.), Living with autism: The individual, the family and the professional. Durham Research Conference, April 1995, Sunderland. pp. 183–193.Google Scholar
  38. Murray, D., Lesser, M., & Lawson, W. (2005). Attention, monotropism and the diagnostic criteria for autism. Autism, 9(2), 136–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Pellicano, E., Dinsmore, A., & Charman, T. (2014). What should autism research focus upon? Community views and priorities from the United Kingdom. Autism, 18(7), 756–770.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314529627.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Sasson, N. J., & Morrison, K. E. (2017). First impressions of adults with autism improve with diagnostic disclosure and increased autism knowledge of peers. Autism.  https://doi.org/10.1777/1362361317729526.
  41. Sasson, N. J., Faso, D. J., Nugent, J., Lovell, S., Kennedy, D. P., & Grossman, R. B. (2017). Neurotypical peers are less willing to interact with those with autism based on thin slice judgments. Scientific Reports, 7, 40700.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Sasson, N. J., Morrison, K. E., Pinkham, A. E., Faso, D. J., & Chmielewski, M. (2018). Brief report: Adults with autism are less accurate at predicting how their personality traits are evaluated by unfamiliar observers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(6), 2243–2248.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Schegloff, E. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97(5), 1295–1345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sheppard, E., Pillai, D., Wong, G. T. L., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2016). How easy is it to read the minds of people with autism spectrum disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 1247–1254.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Stagg, S. D., Slavny, R., Hand, C., Cardoso, A., & Smith, P. (2014). Does facial expressivity count? How typically developing children respond initially to children with autism. Autism, 18(6), 704–711.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Usher, L. V., Burrows, C. A., Messinger, D. S., & Henderson, H. A. (2018). Metaperception in adolescents with and without autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(2), 533–548.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Volker, M. A., Lopata, C., Smith, D. A., & Thomeer, M. L. (2009). Facial encoding of children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 24(4), 195–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. White, S. W., Oswald, D., Ollendick, T., & Scahill, L. (2009). Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(3), 216–229.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Yergeau, M. (2017). Authoring autism: On rhetoric and neurological queerness. Durham: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  • Damian Elgin Maclean Milton
    • 1
    Email author
  • Brett Heasman
    • 2
  • Elizabeth Sheppard
    • 3
  1. 1.Tizard CentreUniversity of KentCanterbury, KentUK
  2. 2.Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE)UCLLondonUK
  3. 3.School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUK