Skip to main content

Informative Value of Vocalizations during Multimodal Interactions in Red-Capped Mangabeys

  • Protocol
  • First Online:
Discovering Hidden Temporal Patterns in Behavior and Interaction

Part of the book series: Neuromethods ((NM,volume 111))

Abstract

Social interactions can be viewed as “dialogs” during which messages between partners can be multimodal, consisting of several simultaneous or sequential signals from different sensory modalities. The nature of a message and the way it will be interpreted may depend on the signals, their order, but also the time intervals between them. This study analyzed the sequential organization of multimodal interactions with and without vocalizations to determine the influence of vocalizations on the behavioral structure of these interactions; to this purpose, we used Theme software which extracts behavioral structures (“patterns”) within behavioral sequences. These behavioral structures are characterized by their temporal organization. Our results on a captive group of red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus) showed that during sociosexual interactions, vocalizations were signals associated with decision-making process of the caller, to initiate contact with a partner some distance away. During grooming interactions, in contrast, vocalizations were not associated with the decision-making process of initiating an interaction. Patterns extracted from interactions with and without calls differed in the number, nature, and position of the decision points they included. So, information conveyed by vocalizations was neither fully original nor redundant with that conveyed by other sensory modalities. Calls seem to reduce complexity of interactions and to decrease the number of decision points partners used to mutually adjust their behaviors. Calls also play a role in structuring interactions as they may occur in patterns either at the beginning or in the “middle” of the interactions but never at the end of them.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Protocol
USD 49.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  1. Weaver W, Shannon CE (1975) Contributions récentes à la théorie mathématique de la communication. In: Retz CE (ed) Théorie mathématique de la communication. C.E.P.L, Paris, pp 29–56

    Google Scholar 

  2. Marler P (1965) Communication in monkeys and apes. In: DeVore I (ed) Primate behavior, field studies of monkeys and apes. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, pp 544–584

    Google Scholar 

  3. Partan S, Marler P (1999) Communication goes multimodal. Science 283:1272–1273

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rowe C, Skelhorn J (2004) Avian psychology and communication. Royal Soc Rev 1435–1442

    Google Scholar 

  5. Burling R (1993) Primate calls, human language, and non verbal communication. Curr Anthropol 34:25–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Altmann SA (1962) Social behavior of anthropoid primates: analysis of recent concepts. In: Bliss EL (ed) Roots of behavior. Harper and Brothers, New York, pp 277–285

    Google Scholar 

  7. Friedman MB (1972) Auditory influences on the reproductive system of the ring dove. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Rutgers–The State University

    Google Scholar 

  8. Friedman MB (1977) Interactions between visual and vocal courtship stimuli in the neuroendocrine response of female doves. J Comp Physiol Psychol 91(6):1408–1416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Fusani L, Hutchison RE, Hutchison JB (1997) Vocal-postural co-ordination of sexually dimorphic display in a monomorphic species: the barbary dove. Behaviour 134:321–335

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Gilliard ET (1969) Birds of paradise and bower birds. Weidenfeld, Nicolson, London

    Google Scholar 

  11. Peek FW (1972) An experimental study of the territorial function of vocal and visual display in the male red-winged blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus). Anim Behav 20:112–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Deputte BL, Goustard M (1980) Copulatory vocalizations of female macaques (Macaca fascicularis): variability factors analysis. Primates 21:83–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Adams DB, Schoel WM (1982) A statistical analysis of the social behavior of the male stumptail macaque (Macaca arctoides). Am J Primatol 2:249–273

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Goldfoot DA (1982) Multiple channels of sexual communication in rhesus monkeys: role of olfactory cues. In: Snowdon CT, Brown CH, Petersen MR (eds) Primate communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 413–428

    Google Scholar 

  15. Partan S (2002) Single and multichannel signal composition: facial expressions and vocalizations of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour 139:993–1027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Smith WJ, Smith SL, DeVilla JG, Oppenheimer EC (1976) The jump-yip display of the black-tailed prairie dog Cynomys ludovicianus. Anim Behav 24:609–621

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hennessy DF, Owings DH, Rowe MP, Coss RG, Leger DW (1981) The information afforded by a variable signal: constraints on snake-elicited tail flagging by California ground squirrels. Behaviour 78:188–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Dienske H, Metz HAJ (1977) Mother-infant body contact in macaques: a time interval analysis. Biol Behav 2:3–37

    Google Scholar 

  19. Pitcairn TK, Schleidt M (1976) Dance and decision, an analysis of a courtship dance of the Medlpa, New Guinea. Behaviour 58:298–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Hinde RA (1966) Animal behaviour: a synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology, 1st edn. McGraw-Hill, London

    Google Scholar 

  21. Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  22. Deputte BL (1986) Ontogenèse du Cercocèbe à joues blanches, en captivité, Lophocebus albigena, Développement des comportements de communication et des relations sociales. Thèse d'état, Université de Rennes I

    Google Scholar 

  23. Altmann SA (1962) A field study of the sociobiology of rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta. Ann N Y Acad Sci 102:338–435

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Altmann SA (1965) Sociobiology of rhesus monkeys. II: Stochastics of social communication. J Theoretical Biol 8:490–522

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Deputte BL (1994) Ethological study of yawning in primates. I. Quantitative analysis and study of causation in two species of Old World monkeys (Cercocebus albigena and Macaca fascicularis). Ethology 98:221–245

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Magnusson MS (1996) Hidden real-time patterns in intra- and interindividual behavior: description and detection. Eur J Psychol Assess 12:112–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–266

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hanby JP, Brown CE (1974) The development of sociosexual behaviours in Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata. Behaviour 49:152–196

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Dawkins R, Dawkins M (1973) Decisions and uncertainty of behaviour. Behaviour 45:83–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Magnusson MS (1989) Structures syntaxiques et rythmes comportementaux: Sur la détection de rythmes cachés. Sciences et Techniques de l’Animal du Laboratoire 14:143–147

    Google Scholar 

  31. Magnusson MS (2000) Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: T-patterns and their detection. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 32(1):93–110

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Beaudichon J, Legros S, Magnusson MS (1991) Organisation des régulations inter et intrapersonnelles dans la transmission d’informations complexes organisées. Bulletin de Psychologie 44(399):110–120

    Google Scholar 

  33. Bernicot J, Caron-Pargue J, Trognon A (1997) Conversation, interaction et fonctionnement cognitif. Collection ‘Processus discursifs, langage et cognition’. Presses Universitaires de Nancy

    Google Scholar 

  34. Féron C (1992) Les comportements socio-sexuels des souris staggerer mâles: caractéristiques et effets de l'expérience sociale. Thèse d'état, Université Paris XIII

    Google Scholar 

  35. Martaresche M, Le Fur C, Magnusson MS, Faure JM, Picard M (2000) Time structure of behavioral patterns related to feed pecking in chicks. Physiol Behav 70:443–451

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Sakaguchi K, Jonsson GK, Hasegawa T (2005) Initial interpersonal attraction between mixed-sex dyad and movement synchrony. In: Anolli L, Jr Duncan S, Magnusson MS, Riva G (eds) The hidden structure of interaction: from neurons to culture patterns. Ios Press, Amsterdam, pp 107–120

    Google Scholar 

  37. Arthur BI Jr, Magnusson MS (2005) Microanalysis of Drosophila courtship behaviour. In: Anolli L, Jr Duncan S, Magnusson MS, Riva G (eds) The hidden structure of interaction: from neurons to culture patterns. Ios Press, Amsterdam, pp 3–22

    Google Scholar 

  38. Magnusson MS (2005) Understanding social interaction: discovering hidden structure with models and algorithms. In: Anolli L, Jr Duncan S, Magnusson MS, Riva G (eds) The hidden structure of interaction: from neurons to culture patterns. Ios Press, Amsterdam, pp 3–22

    Google Scholar 

  39. Maurus M, Pruscha H (1973) Classification of social signals in squirrel monkeys by means of cluster analysis. Behaviour 47:106–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Dennett DC (1983) Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The ‘Panglossian paradigm’ defended. Behav Brain Sci 6:343–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Buck R (1986) The psychology of emotion. In: LeDoux JE, Hirst W (eds) Mind and brain: dialogues in cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 275–300

    Google Scholar 

  42. Hinde RA (1985) Was ‘The expression of the emotions’ a misleading phrase? Anim Behav 33:985–992

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  44. Deputte BL (1993) Ethologie et cognition: le cas des primates. Intellectica 16:21–44

    Google Scholar 

  45. Tavolga WN (1974) Application of the concept of levels of organization to the study of animal communication. In: Krames L, Pliner P, Alloway T (eds) Advances in the study of communication and affect, Vol. 1: Nonverbal communication. Plenum Press, New York, pp 51–76

    Google Scholar 

  46. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2003) Meaning and emotion in animal vocalizations. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1000:32–55

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Bauers KA, de Waal FBM (1991) “Coo” vocalizations in stumptailed macaques: a controlled functional analysis. Behaviour 119:143–160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Brown CH (1986) The perception of vocal signals by blue monkeys and grey-cheeked mangabeys. Exp Biol 45:145–165

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Altmann SA (1967) The structure of primate social communication. In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 325–362

    Google Scholar 

  50. Morton ES (1977) On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. Am Nat 111:855–869

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Weary DM, Fraser D (1995) Signalling need: costly signals and animal welfare assessment. Appl Anim Behav Sci 44:159–169

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank OHLL program for its financial support, Mr Philippe Bec for his help during the handling of the animals. All of the experiments complied with the current laws of the country in which they were conducted (France).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bertrand L. Deputte .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Appendix 1: Examples of Behaviors from the Behavioral Catalogue of Cercocebus T. Torquatus Used for the Study

Except when otherwise mentioned, only subsets of behaviors used to describe mangabeys’ interactions are presented here.

Visual behaviors, including locomotor behaviors, facial expressions, and gaze behaviors:

Locomotor behaviors:

  • To avoid a partner (with a movement of the whole body)

  • To chase a partner in a playful context (pursuit)

  • To chase a partner while displaying aggressive signals

  • To present to a partner = Present, while having the tail up putting the ischial callosities and the callosities against the face of a partner

  • To turn towards a partner: the subject was assuming a sitting posture and ended facing his partner. In addition the subject was within an arm’s reach of his partner

  • To walk away from a partner (the partner was close or in contact with the subject) = to leave

  • To walk towards a partner

Facial expressions:

  • To threaten a partner

  • To lip smack towards a partner from a distance

  • To display a “play face” towards a partner from a distance

Gazes:

  • To follow (≥2 s) a partner with one’s eyes (the partner is moving)

  • To glance (<2 s) at a partner (other body parts than the genitals, callosity, tail, mouth; see below)

  • To glance at what a partner is doing (e.g., looking at fingers of a partner who is eating or grooming or handling an object, etc.)

  • To glance at what a partner is holding (implies that the partner is not doing anything with the held item)

  • To glance backward at a partner

  • To look at a partner (who is motionless) (≥2 s)

  • To look at a partner’s genitals

  • To look at what a partner is doing (looking at fingers of a partner who is eating or grooming or handling an object, etc.)

  • To look at what a partner is holding (see above)

  • To look backward at a partner (who is motionless)

Olfactory behaviors (all the olfactory behaviors used in the study are reported here):

  • To sniff at a partner (on a body part other than mouth, genitals, and tail)

  • To sniff at what a partner holds in his hands

  • To sniff the callosity of a partner

  • To sniff the genitals of a partner

  • To sniff the mouth of a partner

  • To sniff the tail of a partner

Tactile behaviors:

  • To “wrestle play” with a partner

  • To bite a partner

  • To climb up onto a partner

  • To do pelvic movements while mounting a partner (with or without intromission in case of male to female mounting)

  • To embrace a partner

  • To grasp a partner (a body part other than genitals)

  • To groom a partner (on a body part other than genitals or callosity)

  • To hit a partner

  • To lick a partner

  • To touch a partner with hand (somewhere on the partner’s body other than mouth, genitals, callosity, and head)

Appendix 2: Vocal Behaviors (All the Basic Vocalizations of the Species-Specific Repertoire are Reported Here). The Names given to the Vocalizations Correspond to Onomatopoeia (Figs. 6 and 7)

Fig. 6
figure 6

Sonograms (frequency/time display) of 5 common calls from the vocal repertoire of the redcapped mangabey (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus)

Fig. 7
figure 7

Sonograms of 5 additional calls from the vocal repertoire of the red-capped mangabey (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). The Wo call is the red-capped mangabey’s male loud call

Most of the other behaviors correspond to variants of the ones cited here, i.e., precisions of the speed of the locomotor movements (walking, running, jumping), or the position in relation to the partner (back/face/aside), or the body parts of the partner (leg, arm, foot, hand, callosity, genitals, mouth, tail), or the general context of the interaction, i.e., the nature of all the behaviors expressed within a given interaction (playing, aggressive) or the distance between the individual and his partner (from a distance or nearby).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this protocol

Cite this protocol

Baraud, I., Deputte, B.L., Pierre, JS., Blois-Heulin, C. (2016). Informative Value of Vocalizations during Multimodal Interactions in Red-Capped Mangabeys. In: Magnusson, M., Burgoon, J., Casarrubea, M. (eds) Discovering Hidden Temporal Patterns in Behavior and Interaction. Neuromethods, vol 111. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-3249-8_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-3249-8_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4939-3248-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4939-3249-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Protocols

Publish with us

Policies and ethics