Introduction

Autism has reached epidemic proportions, and there is no obvious explanation for this increase [1]. Researchers are scrambling for a reliable aetiology, and the comorbid conditions of autism are largely being overlooked. This is to the detriment of the child with autism, as some of these conditions may be preventable to a certain degree, or at least harbour valid targets for current treatment options. Of great interest is that comorbidities such as cognitive impairment and epilepsy may share a common and dysfunctional neurobiological substrate with autism, leading to the possibility that improvement in one domain may result in more widespread benefit in neurodevelopment. In order for this to happen, certain circuits must be shared by the respective conditions.

If it is considered that the mesial temporal lobes form the main site of epileptogenesis in the brain [2], and that they also constitute the amygdala and hippocampus, which are pivotal to the creation of fear processing, social cognition, and behaviour [3], as well as memory formation and storage [4], then it becomes entirely plausible that epilepsy could be producing the autistic phenotype through the early destruction of this region, called the salience network, and possibly even in utero [5].

Whereas the prevalence of epilepsy in the general childhood population is known and relatively stable (0.5–1.0%), the prevalence of epilepsy in autism remains largely undetermined [6] and currently ranges between 5 and 38%, depending on the selection criteria adopted for each cohort [7, 8]. Similarly, the global prevalence of autism also varies according to geographical distribution, and if a composite of data from both rural and urban areas is taken into account, it is estimated to be 62/10,000 (although many researchers estimate this value to be much higher) [9]. Due to this alarming increase in cases of autism in the world today, and the fact that unregulated seizures seem to accompany the dysfunctional neural circuitry, greater emphasis should be placed on addressing what is both known and treatable.

However, temporal lobe epilepsy is often difficult to diagnose. In making a concrete diagnosis of epilepsy, a patient’s history, clinical examinations, EEG (electroencephalogram), and MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) findings all come together to support the supposition that there is abnormal and ongoing electrical activity in the brain [10].

Certain characteristics specific to the temporal lobe complicate seizure surveillance. A child with temporal lobe seizures may present with sudden and unprovoked temper tantrums [11], or intermittent psychotic episodes [12], which can mimic routine autistic behaviour. Seizures arising within the confines of the temporal lobe may involve visual pathways and result in brief staring episodes [13]. This can be masked by expected anti-social behaviour, as autistic children are known to avoid visual contact and close interpersonal contact. Children with temporal lobe epilepsy often experience seizures at night, which makes their detection difficult and greatly limits parental reporting of seizures [14]. Sleep is a potent driver of epileptogenesis. Melatonin lowers the physiological levels of dopamine during sleep, and dopamine counteracts epilepsy by elevating the seizure threshold [15].

Even the anatomy of the temporal lobes impedes the diagnostic process. As the medial temporal lobes diverge inwardly from the exterior of the brain, discharges arising from these regions are often not detected by the more superficially placed cortical leads applied during EEG [16]. This frequently results in a normal EEG, a misdiagnosis, and a false sense of security. Additionally, the MRIs of the brain often appear normal, even though prolonged epileptogenesis may have occurred [17]. All of these factors make confirmation of the diagnosis of epilepsy elusive and leave both the clinician and the parents of the child in doubt. This can result in delayed treatment and the deterioration in cognition, speech, and behaviour due to unrestrained epileptogenesis.

The dilemma facing clinicians is that seizure semiology in autism, and especially in children, has not been accurately validated [18]. Autism, epilepsy, and cognitive impairment may be comorbid conditions arising from the same defective neurobiological substrate [19]. As a result, clinical presentation, duration, and frequency of seizures in the autistic child may differ from those in normal children. However, all seizure types have previously been reported in children with autism, and autism has not been linked to any particular seizure semiology [20]. The seizure semiology in autism should be accurately characterized in order to facilitate the early detection and prevention of seizure escalation in these already vulnerable children [21].

Lastly, clinical biomarkers are urgently required to identify the autistic child who is at risk of developing epilepsy [22]. By determining the seizure semiology and clinical profile of the epileptic, epileptogenesis can be slowed and possibly even prevented [23]. The fact that seizures may alter and disrupt both cognition and behaviour in the child with autism, then this study of the association between autism and epilepsy, becomes important for both children at risk and their parents [24].

The aim of the study is to raise awareness of the high prevalence of epilepsy in autistic children and to demonstrate the complexities and difficulties of its diagnosis and management in these children.

Methodology

This was a retrospective study that involved 86 children (64 males/22 females) with autism who were followed up at a tertiary level paediatric neurology clinic at Steve Biko Academic Hospital, in the Republic of South Africa, from January 2008 to January 2018. Children were diagnosed with autism based on criteria in the DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-5) and therefore displayed fixed deficits in both social communication and interaction across multiple domains, as well as restricted interests and repetitive patterns of behaviour in activities of daily living [25].

Clinical records relating to the medical history and clinical examination of each child, together with EEG data that had been collected over a 10-year period, were retrospectively analysed in order to determine the prevalence and characteristics of the seizures in these children. EEG data comprised a mixture of 30 min recordings performed during both sleep and wakefulness. EEGs were done by using Niyon Koden EEG-90100/9200 machines from Nihon Kohden Corporation. Japan.

Inclusion criteria

Children who had been diagnosed with autism according to the DSM-5 between the ages of 3 and 13 years old, who had previously displayed fixed deficits in social (non-verbal) and verbal communication, together with restricted interests in activities of daily living, and a preoccupation with purposeless, repetitive behaviours, were included.

Exclusion criteria

The exclusion criteria are as follows:

  1. 1)

    Cases of “syndromic autism”, such as neurocutaneous syndromes (tuberous sclerosis) and other known genetic causes of autism (Angelman syndrome/Rett syndrome).

  2. 2)

    Those with a history of neonatal encephalopathy (Apgar score below 7/cord blood pH below 7.2).

  3. 3)

    Those with microcephaly, cerebral palsy, dysmorphism, or laboratory evidence of a congenital infection.

In the children who were later found to have seizures, four additional clinical parameters were assessed in order to create a clinical profile of the autistic child most at risk of developing epilepsy, namely (1) the level of intellectual disability, (2) history of language and behavioural regression as a young child, (3) the presence of motor stereotypies, and (4) the presence of sleep disturbances, as indicated merely by parental reporting, and without the implementation of formal sleep studies or polysomnography (PSG).

A formal visual-motor assessment (Beery-Buktenica Test) was conducted on each child with epilepsy in order to ascertain their intellectual quotient (IQ) [26]. A Beery Test was performed on the children with epilepsy in order to find a correlation between level of cognition and the presence of epilepsy. The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery Test) evaluates activities of daily living, such as being able to get dressed without assistance, eating and drinking with a cup and spoon, and going to the toilet unsupervised [27]. These measures of executive function are incorporated into the assessment and may be used to determine IQ [28].

Consent for the study was received from the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Pretoria.

Statistical methods

Data was be captured, cleaned, and analysed in Stata: Release 11 (StataCorp. 2009. Statistical Software. College Station, TX, USA: StataCorp LP). Categorical variables were represented as absolute values and percentages. Non-normally distributed numerical variables were represented as medians and interquartile ranges. Normally distributed numerical data were represented as mean and standard deviation. The Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to test differences between numerical variables, and Fisher’s exact test was used to test for differences between categorical variables. A p value of less than 0.05 was used to denote statistical significance.

Sampling and sample size

This retrospective study made use of opportunistic sampling to obtain the required sample size. Opportunistic sampling was used as the prevalence of epilepsy is low (between 0.5 and 1%), and it would be the best sampling method to reach the required sample size.

For the proposed cross sectional study, using the formula n = Z2P (1 − P)/d2 where n denotes the sample size, Z denotes the statistic corresponding to the level of confidence, P denotes the expected prevalence, and d denotes the level of precision with d = 0.05 and prevalence of 0.01, a total of 16 participants with epilepsy would be required.

Results

The prevalence of epilepsy in the autistic children was 23% (17 boys and 3 girls). The median age of the children with epilepsy was 7 years (IQR 5.5 to 9): 7 years (IQR 6 to 8) for the male children and 6 years (IQR 5 to 12) for the female children (p value 0.96).

Staring episodes, lasting either seconds or a few minutes, occurred in 85% of the children with epilepsy, and they were always the first manifestation in the series of ictal events. Lateral version of the eyes occurred in 60% of the children, and a few also displayed recurrent blinking of the eyelids at the onset of a seizure. Nineteen patients (95%) presented with at least one of these ocular signs during the initial stages of the seizure. In 55% of the patients with epilepsy, seizures would only occur at night during sleep.

Generalized tonic-clonic seizures were observed in 11 children (55%). Only two children displayed focal semiology involving the limbs on one side of the body. Although seizure frequency could only be determined in 15 of the 20 children with epilepsy, due to either absent guardians at follow-up visits or unreliable record keeping, 60% of the patients with adequately maintained seizure charts would only have seizures on 2 or 3 days over a 1- to 6-month period. The relative infrequency of seizures may complicate the detection of seizures in autism.

Although the age of seizure onset could not be determined in all cases, eight of the epileptic children developed seizures during infancy, and five of these had seizure-onset within the first 6 months of life. Epilepsy had been confirmed in 11 children before the age of 3 years, at which time autism is often first diagnosed.

All seizure types were noted, and no specific seizure type could be reliably linked to autism. In toddlers, seizures would manifest in the form of apnoeic episodes, breath-holding spells, and sudden unprovoked temper tantrums. Drop attacks (atonic seizures) involving the head, neck, and shoulders and occurring in clusters were often accompanied by other seizure types, similar to that which occurs in other epileptic encephalopathies. However, not all seizure types were atypical, and one child presented with typical absence epilepsy. Unusual semiology was also noted, as evidenced by a child with gelastic seizures (recurrent bouts of impersonal laughter) that occurred routinely in the early hours of the morning, and another who experienced a very rare form of epilepsy, namely cursive seizures, that resulted in the child to getting up abruptly and running out of the house.

Five children (25%) endured recurrent episodes of status epilepticus (seizures lasting longer than 20 min). Four of these patients with recurrent status epilepticus had persistently normal EEGs (Table 1).

Table 1 Severity of epilepsy amongst autistic children

The majority of the EEGs (55%) demonstrated a focal, as opposed to a generalized epileptiform dysfunction. In five EEGs, the epileptiform focus was situated in the frontal lobes. The seizures originated from the temporal lobe in three children and from the occipital lobe in another. Two patients had multifocal epileptiform dysfunction. Only two of the EEGs (10%) revealed a generalized epileptiform dysfunction.

The EEGs of three children revealed a non-specific dysfunction which may have been epileptiform but which did not demonstrate typical spike-wave discharges. These were in the form of runs of either theta slow-wave or beta fast-wave activity in the frontal or temporal regions, which may have preceded epileptiform discharges. One EEG showed the absence of regular interictal alpha wave rhythms during wakefulness in one hemisphere, which may indicate the presence of an underlying epileptiform dysfunction. In four children, the EEG was normal on more than one occasion, in the presence of recurrent and unprovoked clinical seizures. There was no statistically significant association between EEG findings and clinical semiology (OR 0.29 to 40.95, p value 0.33). Whereas a significant proportion of the EEGs were focal, most clinical seizures were generalized.

All of the children with epilepsy (100%) had intellectual disability (ID) with an IQ below 80 (Table 2).

Table 2 Clinical profile of autistic children with epilepsy

Of great importance is that 45% of these had severe (IQ 20–40) to profound (IQ < 20) ID. The three girls in the group all had severe to profound intellectual disability with an IQ below 40 (Table 3).

Table 3 IQ level of autistic children with epilepsy

Females used a significantly higher number of drugs (4, IQR 3 to 4) than the males (1, IQR 1 to 3) (p value 0.03) (Table 4). Two thirds of the children who had been placed on two or more anti-epileptic drugs had severe to profound intellectual disability.

Table 4 Number of drugs used by autistic children with epilepsy

A history of regression was an unreliable marker of epilepsy. Only a single child had a history of language or developmental regression. With regard to the presence of motor stereotypies as a possible marker of epilepsy, 12 children (60%) with epilepsy demonstrated recurrent, purposeless, or stereotypical behaviour. Six of them displayed primarily hand-flapping, and another six demonstrated recurrent rocking of either the head or torso and a preoccupation with turning around in circles. All of those who engaged in rocking behaviour had a level of cognition that was severely to profoundly impaired (IQ below 40).

Half of the children with epilepsy experienced sleep disturbances at night (either in the form of delayed sleep-onset or recurrent nocturnal awakenings), which was well within the expected limits experienced by autistic children without epilepsy. Sleep disturbances therefore could not be used as a reliable marker of epilepsy.

Discussion

Over the last few years, there has been a surge in interest within the field of autism research regarding the rising incidence of autism within the childhood population [29]. It is widely postulated that a continuous interaction exists between genetic and epigenetic [30] or environmental factors which play out on a second-by-second basis. With all this focus on aetiology, the comorbidities linked with autism have been progressively overlooked. Regular reporting of different patient cohorts is becoming more and more necessary in determining the natural history and developmental characteristics of the disorder [31].

The prevalence of epilepsy in autism is extremely high (23% in this study). The seizures in autism are subtle, nocturnal, and infrequent. Although staring represents a complex partial seizure, the significance of this type of seizure in autism is not known, and this is relevant when choosing anti-convulsant treatment [32]. They often occur in infancy and can simulate autistic behaviour. This implies that they are unrecognized and underreported. In fact, the relative frequency of the various types of seizure disorders in autism is unknown [33], and there has been surprisingly little study of the relationship between staring episodes and temporal lobe epilepsy in children in general [34]. Very few studies are available that attempt to create a clinical profile for the child with autism who may be having occult seizures late at night. Further studies are needed to determine modifiable risk factors for non-febrile seizures in autism [35]. If left untreated, the seizures can accelerate epileptogenesis, resulting in cognitive deficits and worsening behaviour. Early diagnosis and intervention are therefore critical.

The epilepsy in autism is also severe. In many children, the epilepsy becomes intractable, and in most cases, two or more anti-epileptic drugs are used to control the seizures. The epilepsy is dangerous; a significant number of children present with recurrent episodes of status epilepticus, which has an ominous mortality rate attached to it [36, 37]. Herein lies the paradox when dealing with epilepsy in autism; it appears early and often remains concealed during the early stages of development.

Any seizure type can present in autism, and the same seems to apply to the EEG [38]. To complicate matters, the EEG often does not correlate with the seizure semiology. The MRI scans are often reported as being normal, and parenchymal lesions are not responsible for the epilepsy in the vast majority of cases [39]. The resulting paucity of auxiliary evidence creates doubt in the mind of the attending physician as to if epilepsy is indeed present or not. Similarly, a history of developmental regression is not a reliable marker of epilepsy in autism [40].

It has also been shown that a similar developmental trajectory exists between autistic children who underwent a period of regression and those who did not [41]. There are studies that state that epilepsy is associated with autistic regression in children [42, 43]. Other studies contradict this and state that epilepsy is a comorbidity of autism and does not contribute to the initiation of a period of loss in previously acquired milestones [44]. Despite the occurrence of clinical seizures, the presence or absence of developmental regression has no predictive value or influence on the epileptiform nature of these EEGs [45]. The truth probably lies somewhere in between, and epilepsy plays a minor but not negligible role in autistic regression [46]. The neurobiological basis of regression needs to be investigated and characterized, as there still may be a minor but non-negligible association with epilepsy.

The prevalence and nature of stereotypical behaviour may be an “indirect” marker of epilepsy in autism. The more robust the clinical presentation of a particular comorbidity, namely autism, epilepsy, and mental retardation, within an individual, the greater the likelihood will be that stereotypical behaviour will occur [47]. Hand and finger stereotypies, and in particular hand flapping, seem to occur more often in higher-functioning children, whereas self-stimulatory behaviour (in the form of head nodding, body rocking, and, in some cases, episodes of hyperventilation) appears to be more prevalent in lower-functioning children [48].

As the nature and presence of motor stereotypical behaviour can reflect the level of cognition, and as lower levels of cognition are more associated with epilepsy, self-stimulatory behaviour will be more likely to occur in severely cognitively impaired children with epilepsy. Epilepsy can also mimic stereotypical behaviour and aggravate existing motor stereotypies [49]. Motor stereotypies are also associated with the severity of progression in autistic individuals, beyond that predicted by social communication measures alone [50], although symptomatology in autism may change with age [51]. Therefore, further studies are required that can elucidate reliable and quantitative measurements of autistic symptoms [52].

The ratio of males to females in the total cohort of children with autism was 3.75:1, which is in keeping with international estimates [53]. All of the females who had epilepsy had an IQ below 40 and were severely to profoundly cognitively impaired. Each one of them had intractable epilepsy and was on at least three or more anti-epileptics. The above factors seem to indicate that epilepsy severity is directly proportional to the level of intellectual disability. There is a need for a detailed neuropsychiatric assessment in all children who present with the combination of epilepsy, learning disability, and autistic behaviour [54]. The risk of epilepsy is proportionate to the degree of underlying brain dysfunction [55].

Each female had definitive epileptiform spikes or sharp waves on her EEG and had been referred for MRI. Of critical importance is that all of them had their first seizures during infancy. In two of these children, seizure onset was below the age of 6 months. There was also a strong correlation between early seizure onset and the later development of recurrent episodes of status epilepticus. Therefore, female gender could be a useful marker for epilepsy. Remarkably little research has focused on the reasons for this disparity in gender. It remains to be seen whether or not the female brain is vulnerable to the effects of epileptogenesis in autism. Better understanding of this gender difference could lead to major advancements in the prevention of the autistic phenotype in both males and females [56].

This study aims to explore a possible association between autism and epilepsy. Such an undertaking is important for a number of reasons. Very few systematic investigations that explore the link between autism and epilepsy have been done. Danielsson et al. conducted the largest and longest prospective population-based study to establish the prevalence and characteristics of epilepsy in autism. They followed up 120 autistic individuals (diagnosed in childhood) to determine how many of them would develop epilepsy in childhood and how many would undergo remission in adulthood [57]. The landmark study was published in Epilepsia and highlighted the need for similar studies from different geographical locations, in order to “neutralize” the effect which the environment may exert on susceptible individuals.

Conclusion

Brain wiring is abnormal in autism. Cognitive impairment and epilepsy share the same dysfunction in messaging. Epilepsy, therefore, is extremely common in autism. Not only does it delay neurodevelopment but it worsens behaviour. This has a huge impact not only on the individual but also on the whole family. If left unchecked, the seizures become intractable, and numerous anti-epileptics are utilized, thereby increasing the side-effect profile. As these seizures can mimic autistic behaviour, a greater awareness must be fostered throughout the autism community. This is one of only a few such studies to have come out of Africa. While the world rushes to identify modifiable risk factors in autism, the significance of epilepsy should not be overlooked.

Study limitations

Due to the fact that autism, epilepsy, and mental retardation overlap, the results of the study may reflect the selection bias of the group. It was difficult to ascertain which had arisen first, autism or epilepsy in two children. This is due to the fact that the epilepsy in both of these children started in infancy. However, the parents noticed from an early age that the pervasive symptoms were dominant and occurred alongside the epilepsy, and not as a consequence thereof.

Autistic regression is determined by three factors, namely language regression or a loss of communication skills, loss of social skills, and onset of restricted and repetitive behaviour. Parents only reported language regression; however, the loss of social skills and the development of motor stereotypies are as important when assessing regression. The prevalence of regression and its association with epilepsy may have been significantly higher had all three aspects been considered.