Introduction

Evidence supporting a direct association between the Apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE4) and the risk for age-related cognitive decline is growing; APOE4 status (i.e., having 1 or 2 ε4 alleles vs. none) is among the most well-established genetic risk factors for late onset Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and for age-related cognitive decline [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22]. Although this association is generally consistent, some studies indicated that APOE4 may in fact reduce the risk for adverse cognitive outcomes, others failed to detect an association, and yet others found this relationship only among dementia patients [7,8,9,10]. Furthermore, whether the association of APOE4 with cognitive decline is specific to certain domains of cognition, such as verbal memory, is still unclear as is whether socio-demographic factors such as race act as important effect modifiers in that relationship.

Previous studies have detected direct associations between APOE4 and impairment or decline on domains of episodic memory [5, 11], verbal fluency [12], executive functioning [13, 14], perceptual/psychomotor speed, and visuo-spatial skill [11, 16, 17], as well as global mental status [9, 20]. Effect modification by sex in the relationship between APOE and cognitive outcomes has been investigated in several cross-sectional and longitudinal studie s[5, 6, 12, 23,24,25,26,27,28]. Earlier meta-analysis suggested sex differences in the APOE genotype-Alzheimer’s disease (AD) association [29], but several experimental and neurobiological studies indicated that the impact of APOE4 on neurodegeneration was more tangible among women compared to men [27, 30]. The studies that have examined associations between APOE and cognitive change thus far have been conducted using samples comprised largely of participants of European ancestry. Thus, race may also play an important role in this association, given the distributional differences in the APOE genotype by race, particularly between individuals of European and African ancestries. Only few studies have examined these associations in diverse population to understand how they may vary across racial groups. Most studies focused on the outcomes of incident AD or single domain (or global) cognitive decline (e.g., [3, 15]). It is worth noting that the association between race and cognitive decline may be more reflective of race as a social construct as opposed to ancestry. This is not the case for APOE genotype, which is largely determined by race as an ancestry construct. Importantly, there is a gap in the literature as to which domains of cognition are most affected by APOE genotypes differentially by race, as well as by sex within each racial group. The association of APOE ε2 (APOE2) allelic dosage with cognition, generally found protective against cognitive decline, also remains under-studied [31,32,33,34] especially in terms of race- and sex effect modifications.

The objectives of the present study were to (i) evaluate the associations of APOE2 and APOE4 allelic dosages with cognitive performance and change over time and (ii) explore racial differentials in those associations. As a secondary objective, (iii) we examined sex differences in those associations, overall, and within each racial group. We used data from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study which consisted of African American and White men and women with baseline ages 30–64 years [35]. The HANDLS study was uniquely designed to study health disparities and contains an extant battery of cognitive measures as well as APOE genotype information, rendering it useful for addressing our research questions.

Methods

Study design

Our sample was drawn from the HANDLS study, an ongoing prospective cohort study of socioeconomically diverse African American and White men and women in Baltimore, MD [35]. Baseline data collection took place from 2004 to 2009 and was conducted in two phases. Phase I consisted of information collected from screening, recruitment, and a household interview which included a 24-h dietary recall [35]. Phase II involved an in-person physical health assessment which included, for example, a complete physical examination, an electrocardiogram, and a detailed cognitive battery [35]. Participants were invited to participate in a follow-up in-person assessment between 2009 and 2013. In addition to physical health measures, the HANDLS investigators also collected clinical and molecular biomarkers that span multiple physiological systems [35]. Written informed consent was obtained for all participants. The HANDLS study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences [35].

Vital status in HANDLS was ascertained through linkage to the National Death Index (NDI), National Center for Health Statistics. The HANDLS-linked records include underlying cause of death in addition to other conditions or causes of death listed on the death certificate classified using the International Statistical Classification of Diseases, Version 10 (ICD-10) codes. Vital status information for all participants is available from enrollment (2004–2009) through December 31, 2018.

In this study, we utilized up to two repeats on cognitive test scores from v1 and/or v2 along with exposure data on APOE genotypes available for a sub-sample of Whites and African Americans participating in HANDLS, while excluding individuals who did not survive within a year of follow-up. Specifically, among 3720 initially recruited participants, we excluded those who died within 1 year of their baseline interview (n = 35) in the main analysis to ensure at least 12 months of follow-up; and then further excluded 1339 individuals for whom APOE genotype information was unavailable resulting in an analytic sample of 2346 individuals. Finally, we excluded 576 participants with missing or non-credible cognitive test information at both visits for all tests, which yielded an analytic sample of 1770 individuals, with an average number of observations/participant (k = 1.7), indicating 15% missingness on cognitive test performance outcomes (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participant flowchart. Abbreviations: APOE = Apolipoprotein E; HANDLS = Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span

The inclusion criteria resulted in significant differences in characteristics of the samples relative to the complete baseline sample (n = 3720). In fact, individuals selected into the sample with complete cognitive test scores, APOE genotype exposures, and other non-covariate exclusions (n = 1770) were slightly older (mean [standard error]: 48.5 [0.22] vs 47.9 [0.22], p = 0.0308), more likely to be female (57.2% vs 52.6%, p = 0.006), less likely to be below poverty (38.9% vs 43.3%, p = 0.006), and more likely to be White (44.9% vs 37.4%, p < 0.001) than those excluded.

Outcomes: cognitive measures

HANDLS researchers assessed cognitive function using a battery of tests which included the following: the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), California Verbal Learning Test–List A (CVLT-List-A); California Verbal Learning Test–Free Recall Long Delay (CVLT-DFR), Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT), Brief Test of Attention (BTA), Animal Fluency Test (AF), Digits Span Forward Test (DS-F), Digits Span Backward Test (DS-B), Clock Draw Test (CDT), Trailmaking Test A (Trails A), and Trailmaking Test B (Trails B). MMSE total score was considered as an outcome both in its initial scale and as normalized by using previously described methods [36]. Detailed description of each cognitive test score and the related domains are available as supplemental information (Method S1).

Exposures: APOE allelic dosages

The APOE gene is coded by three common alleles (ε2, ε3, ε4) which form six genotypes (ε2/ε2, ε2/ε3, ε2/ε4, ε3/ε3, ε3/ ε4, ε4/ε4). In the current study, APOE genotype was determined on the basis of two variants (rs429358 [APOE-C112R], rs7412) [APOE-R158C]). Genotyping of these variants was achieved with the Taqman Assays (Applied Biosystems Assay-On-Demand part numbers C__3084793_20 and C__904973_10) on a 7900HT Sequence Detection System (Applied Biosystems). Detailed information on assay procedures are available elsewhere [37]. We classified APOE allelic dosage separately for ε2 and ε4 on the basis of having zero, one, or two of the aforementioned alleles. In both cases, the ε3/ε3 genotype acquired a value of zero. For APOE2 dosage, ε3/ε4 and ε4/ε4, also acquired a value of zero. For APOE4 dosage, ε2/ε2 and ε2/ε3 were also given a value of zero. APOE2 dosage had a value of 2 for ε2/ε2, while APOE4 dosage had a value of 2 for ε4/ε4. Thus, a value of 1 was ascribed to APOE2 dosage for the ε2/ε3 or ε2/ε4 genotypes, while a value of 1 was ascribed to APOE4 dosage for ε2/ε4 or ε3/ε4 genotypes.

Covariates

This study considered among potential confounders several covariates for their documented association with cognitive performance or decline, which may also be associated with APOE2 or APOE4 dosage exposures. These included age at v1 (continuous, years), sex (male, female), race (White, African American), poverty status (below vs. above 125% the federal poverty line), educational attainment (less than high school, high school, more than high school), and literacy (Wide Range Achievement Test, third edition [WRAT-3]). Age at v2 was also used to compute time between visits 1 and 2. Poverty status was categorized by using the US Census Bureau poverty thresholds for 2004 relying on self-reported household income and total family size including children under age 18 years, with < 125% of the 2004 Health and Human Services poverty guidelines labelled as “below poverty” and ≥ 125% as “above poverty” [38]. Additionally, lifestyle and health-related factors were considered among potentially confounding covariates. Those measures included illicit drug use (0 = No vs. 1 = Yes, using any of marijuana, opiates, and cocaine), current smoking status (0 = No vs. 1 = Yes), body mass index (BMI, weight/height2 , kg m−2, continuous), self-rated health status coded 0 = poor/average (referent), 1 = good and 2 = very good/excellent, the Healthy Eating Index 2010 (HEI-2010), a measure of food and macronutrient-based overall diet quality, total energy intake (kcal/day), and the total score on the 20-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale (CES-D), reflecting depressive symptoms. Furthermore, we accounted for an unweighted co-morbidity index composed of hypertension (0 = no, 1 = yes), diabetes (0 = diabetic, 1 = pre-diabetic, 2 = diabetic) and dyslipidemia (or statin use) (0 = no, 1 = yes), and self-reported history of any of several cardiovascular disease conditions (0 = no, 1 = yes). The latter component accounted for the occurrence of any of atrial fibrillation, angina, coronary artery disease, congestive heart failure, and myocardial infarction. Thus, the index’s potential range was between 0 and 5. Many of these measures, including poverty status and HEI-2010 are detailed elsewhere [39].

Statistical analyses

We used Stata release 16 [40] to conduct all analyses. We described the selected sample at baseline, by utilizing means and proportions, and using t-tests to examine racial differences in those characteristics. To examine racial differences in continuous, binary, and categorical multi-level covariates, we used linear, logistic, multinomial logit, and by conducting linear mixed-effect models with TIME on study to examine the relationship between race and rate of change in cognitive performance. We ran both unadjusted models, and models adjusted for age, sex, and poverty status (Method S2). In terms of the main hypotheses, we tested cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between APOE allelic dosages and cognitive performance over time (i.e., baseline performance and annual rates of change), by conducting multiple mixed-effects linear regression models (Method S2), with TIME on study being considered the underlying time scale. Random effects were added to the intercept and the TIME variable, which was interacted with APOE allelic dosages (ε2 or ε4), as well as with all potentially confounding covariates, to test the covariate-adjusted associations of APOE allelic dosages with annual rates of change in cognitive performance. The main effects of APOE2 or APOE4 dosage exposures were included in the model—as were the main effects of other covariates—to examine the covariate-adjusted association of APOE2 or APOE4 dosage exposures with baseline cognitive performance. Modeling consisted of first fitting a minimally adjusted model (Model 1), with covariates included being baseline age, sex, race, and poverty status. Model 2 adjusted for all other socio-demographic, lifestyle, and health-related factors listed in the “Covariates” section, some of which could be considered as potential mediators. Nevertheless, we chose covariates related with cognitive performance trajectories in prior research. Due to missing data on many of these covariates (< 5% missing, on average) and to preserve sample size between reduced and full models, multiple imputations were carried out (5 imputations, 10 iterations) using the chained equations method. All covariates were utilized simultaneously in this estimation process, as was done in previous studies and continuous covariates were centered to their means [39]. Thus, Models 1 and 2 were applied to 2 exposures (APOE2 and APOE4 allelic dosages), 11 cognitive test scores (v1 cognitive performance and cognitive performance change over time), one main stratifying variable (race), and a secondary stratifying variable (sex within each racial group). Racial differences in the association between APOE allelic dosages and cognitive performance at v1 was tested using APOE2 or APOE4 × Race interaction terms in separate models, while that of the association between APOE allelic dosages and cognitive change was carried out by testing the APOE2 or APOE4 × TIME × Race term in the same model. In order to assess sex differences within each racial group, other models were conducted whereby sex was interacted with APOE2 or APOE4 and APOE2 or APOE4 × TIME among Whites and African Americans, separately. Sex differences overall were also assessed using a similar approach with 2-way and 3-way interaction terms (with APOE dosage exposures and TIME) and with sex instead of race.

We used a two-stage Heckman selection strategy for the mixed-effects linear regression models, thus partially accounting for sample selectivity. To this end, we first regressed an indicator of selection on age at baseline, sex, race, and poverty status using a probit model; this yielded an inverse mills ratio (IMR). At a second stage, we estimated our mixed-effects regression models adjusted for the IMR in addition to aforementioned covariates [39].

This study set the type I error rate a priori for main and interactive effects before correction for multiple testing to 0.05 and 0.10, respectively [41]. We accounted for outcome multiplicity (i.e., 11 cognitive test scores) using the approach of familywise Bonferroni correction [42], specifically for Model 1. As such, Model 2 was considered a sensitivity model in which potentially confounding and/or mediating factors were added. In this context, we adjusted significance levels for main effects to p < 0.0045 (0.05/11). Significance levels for the two-way interaction terms were adjusted to 0.10/11 = 0.009, as was done in previously published work [39], given the reduced statistical power of 2-way interactions [41]. The main findings were also presented as predictive margins of cognitive performance outcomes across follow-up time, within each APOE2 or APOE4 stratum and by race or race by sex. Standardized regression coefficients (“b”) for the APOE by TIME parameter was also presented and interpreted as the number of SD of cognitive performance change (for each group, i.e., race, sex, or sex by race), per 1 year change in follow-up time and 1 dosage increase in the APOE exposure. This measure can be extrapolated to a 10-year follow-up period for ease of interpretation. Given that the outcome is standardized, this can be used as a measure of effect size over a 10-year follow-up period. A sensitivity analysis was also conducted for all eligible participants, including those who died within 12 months of follow-up.

Results

Table 1 displays study sample characteristics, overall, and by race in the final analytic sample. Overall, the vast majority of participants were non-ε2 (80.5%) and/or non-ε4 (66.7%) allele carriers. African Americans were more likely than Whites to be APOE4 carriers, including the ε4/ε4 genotype, a pattern also observed for APOE2 carrier status, specifically for the APOE2 dosage of 1. In fact, the ε3/ε3 genotype was significantly more common in Whites vs. African Americans (61.1% vs. 43.0%, p < 0.05). African Americans had a higher likelihood than Whites to be living below poverty (45% vs. 31%), coupled with a lower percentage above HS level of education (34% vs. 36%). They also had a lower mean for WRAT-3 literacy score (40.8 vs. 44.8 among Whites, p < 0.05). Racial differences, though inconsistent in terms of directionality, were also detected with respect to current drug use, self-rated health, depressive symptoms, hypertension, dyslipidemia, and cardiovascular disease. Cognitive test scores were generally suggestive of poorer performance among African Americans as opposed to Whites. Nevertheless, the pace of decline among African Americans was faster than among Whites only in the case of BVRT, and independently of age, sex, and poverty status. Furthermore, time on study among those with complete visit 2, differed significantly by race, with African Americans having a longer mean follow-up time compared to Whites (Mean ± SE, years: 4.78 ± 0.04 vs. 4.29 ± 0.03, p < 0.001).

Table 1 Study sample characteristics, overall, and by race in final analytic sample with imputed covariates (N = 1770), HANDLS 2004–2013

Table 2 presents the findings from a series of linear mixed-effects regression models examining the associations of APOE2 or APOE4 allelic dosages with cognitive performance over time. These associations were tested overall and by race, and heterogeneity by sex was also tested within each racial category. After correction for multiple testing, most of the associations were deemed nonsignificant. Only one association passed the familywise Bonferroni correction criterion. The latter indicated that APOE4 dosage was associated with faster decline on a test of verbal memory, immediate recall among Whites only (CVLT-List A, TIME × APOE4, among Whites, γ12 = − 0.370 ± 0.139, p = 0.008), in the minimally adjusted model (i.e., Model 1). This association remained virtually unaltered after further adjustment for additional socio-demographic, lifestyle, and health-related factors (γ12 = − 0.364 ± 0.137, p = 0.008) and was significantly different between Whites and African Americans (CVLT-List A, TIME×APOE4 × Race, overall sample, p < 0.05). A marginal association (P < 0.05) in the same direction that did not pass correction for multiple testing was also found between APOE4 and CVLT-DFR annual rate of change, also among Whites only with a significant difference by race, indicating an adverse effect of APOE4 dosage on memory decline within that racial group in particular. This association was also unaltered by additional adjustment for important potential confounders. The difference in trajectories of CVLT-List A across APOE4 dosages among Whites and African Americans are shown in Fig. 2. These figures include a measure of standardized regression coefficient for the annualized rate of change in CVLT-List A, whereby the cognitive outcome was entered as a standardized z-score. This effect can be interpreted as a − 0.50 SD difference in CVLT-List A score after change a 10-year follow-up period (b = 0.050 for a 1-year follow-up), contrasting APOE4 dosages of “1” vs. “0” or “2” vs. “1,” among Whites. This effect size is reduced to + 0.005 SD among African Americans after 10 years of follow-up. All SDs for 11 cognitive performance tests over the period of follow-up (v1 through v2), and in the overall eligible sample (N = 1770), are presented in Figure S1 to contextualize the effects observed of APOE exposures on cognitive performance and change in the mixed-effects linear regression models.

Table 2 APOE2 and APOE4 allelic dosages and their association with cognitive performance at v1 and change over time: overall and race-specific mixed-effects linear regression models: HANDLS 2004–2013
Fig. 2
figure 2

APOE4 allelic dosage vs. CVLT-List A trajectories among Whites and African Americans: predictive margins from mixed-effects linear regression modela. Abbreviations: APOE = Apolipoprotein E; CVLT-List A = California Verbal Learning Test, List A; HANDLS = Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span. a Model 2B, Table 2, fully adjusted model, stratified by race. Figure shows predictive margins of CVLT-List A scores across time, highlighting the significant difference in slope across APOE4 dosage among Whites only. γ12 refers to the fixed effect of APOE4 dosage on the rate of change in CVLT-List A total score for each racial group. “b” is a standardized measure for the regression coefficient γ12 in model where CVLT-LIST A is entered as standardized z-scores within each racial group. It is interpreted as the SD of outcome change per year increase in follow-up time for each APOE4 dosage change. 1 SD of CVLT-LIST A corresponds to 7.71 change in score, overall

Other important associations that were detected in race-stratified models (Table 2) prior to correction for multiple testing included an inverse association between APOE2 dosage and MMSE annual rate of change, overall, and in both models and a direct relationship between APOE4 dosage and BTA annual rate of change among African Americans (Models 1 and 2). Moreover, among Whites, TRAILS B performance at v1 was poorer with increased APOE4 dosage, while annual rate of change in TRAILS B with APOE4 dosage indicated an opposing trend (i.e., improvement over time). This was not the case among African Americans [TRAILS B (overall sample), APOE4 × Race, APOE4 × TIME × Race, P < 0.05].

Findings from the secondary analyses, stratified by sex, are shown in Table S1. The results indicate that there were no detected sex-specific associations between APOE2 or APOE4 dosages and cognitive performance at v1 or decline over time, after adjustment for multiple testing.

Sex differences in associations of APOE2 or APOE4 allelic dosages with cognitive performance over time were noted within each racial group (Table 2 and S2). Among Whites, one association (APOE2 dosage vs. change in BTA over time) was more pronounced in women (APOE2 × TIME × Sex, p < 0.05). Specifically, among White women, APOE2 allelic dosage was directly associated with annual rate of change in BTA, reflecting a protective effect (γ11 = + 0.145 ± 0.070, p = 0.039, Model 1; γ11 = + 0.160 ± 0.069, p = 0.020, Model 2), an association not detected among White men. Among African Americans, several other associations between APOE2 or APOE4 dosages and cognitive performance over time differed between men and women. Most notably, APOE4 dosage was directly associated with annual rate of change in BTA, suggestive of improvement over time in the domain of attention, among African American women (γ12 = + 0.114 ± 0.035, p = 0.001, Model 1; γ12 = + 0.106 ± 0.035, p = 0.002, Model 2) and passing correction for multiple testing, although that relationship was not detected among men (p > 0.10). The contrast in BTA trajectories across APOE4 dosages based on Model 2 between African American women and men is displayed in Fig. 3. Similar to Figs. 2, the effect sizes are shown in Figs. 3, indicating that after a 10-year follow-up, a one dosage increase in APOE4 is associated with a + 0.50 SD (b = 0.050 for a 1 year follow-up) difference in change in BTA score among African American women, as opposed to a − 0.0014 SD difference in change in BTA among African American men after 10 years of follow-up (see Figure S1 for SDs of cognitive performance test scores).

Fig. 3
figure 3

APOE4 allelic dosage vs. BTA trajectories among African American women and men: predictive margins from mixed-effects linear regression modela. Abbreviations: APOE = Apolipoprotein E; BTA = Brief Test of Attention; HANDLS = Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span. a Model 2E, Table 2, fully adjusted model, stratified by race and sex. Figure shows predictive margins of BTA scores across time with their 95% CI, highlighting the significant difference in slope across APOE4 dosage among African American women only. γ12 refers to the fixed effect of APOE4 dosage on the rate of change in BTA total score for each sex group among African Americans. “b” is a standardized measure for the regression coefficient γ12 in model where BTA is entered as standardized z-scores within each racial/sex group. It is interpreted as the SD of outcome change per year increase in follow-up time for each APOE4 dosage change. 1 SD of BTA corresponds to 6.63 change in score, overall

Finally, among African American men (unlike among women), both APOE2 and APOE4 dosages were directly linked to annual rates of change in CDT, suggestive of improvement over time in the visuo-spatial abilities domain (p < 0.10 for γ11 and γ12 in Model 1), though these associations did not survive correction for multiple testing. They were also markedly attenuated in Model 2, mainly due to adjustment for current smoking and drug use status and HEI-2010 total score. Our key findings were not altered when a sensitivity analysis was conducted with all eligible participants, including those that died within 12 months of follow-up (n < 10 for each analysis).

Discussion

Summary of findings

Our present work is among few longitudinal studies to examine racial differences in the associations of APOE2 and APOE4 dosages with cognitive performance over time, particularly longitudinal change in test scores spanning key domains of cognition in sample of middle-aged urban adults. We observed several key findings. Upon correction for multiple testing, socio-demographic, lifestyle, and health-related potential confounders, APOE4 allelic dosage was associated with faster decline on a test of verbal memory among Whites only (CVLT-List A: γ12 = − 0.363 ± 0.137, p = 0.008), but not among African Americans. In contrast, among African American women, a higher APOE4 dosage (from 0 to 2) was linked to slower decline on a test of attention (BTA: γ12 = + 0.106 ± 0.035, p = 0.002), while no such association was detected among African American men with respect to APOE4 dosage and change in BTA over time. APOE2 and APOE4 dosages showed inconsistent results in other domains of cognition overall and across racial groups that did not survive correction for multiple testing.

Previous studies

Prior work evaluating associations between APOE genotype and cognitive health has also reported heterogeneity in the magnitude of these associations across racial/ethnic groups and depending on the phenotype under study. For example, several studies [1,2,3,4, 15] have reported larger associations between the ε4 allele and incident AD dementia among European Americans compared to their African American counterparts yet relatively little work has examined variation in cognitive decline—as opposed to incident dementia—among European and African American carriers of the ε4 allele. In a recent study by Rajan and colleagues [43], the authors examined disparities in the association between the ε4 allele and cognitive decline among European and African American men and women using a composite measure of cognitive function that included assessments of episodic memory, perceptual speed, executive function, general orientation, and global cognition. The authors found that, in addition to being more likely to have at least one ε4 allele, African American adults also had lower cognitive function at baseline in addition to accelerated cognitive decline over nearly 10 years of follow-up compared to European Americans in the sample [43]. Moreover, carriers of the ε4 allele—irrespective of ancestry—were more likely to exhibit rapid and moderate cognitive decline relative to slow decline [43]. This finding differs from ours, as we mainly found baseline differences in cognitive performance (better performance among Whites compared with African Americans) in most tests, with only one test showing differences in terms of decline (BVRT), whereby Whites had slower decline than African Americans. In addition, our finding of a direct association between the ε4 allele dosage and decline on a test of verbal memory was limited to Whites, unlike the study by Rajan and colleagues [43]. In the latter study [43], genotypic frequencies and differences by race were comparable to our sample, with African Americans’ vs. Whites’ frequencies for each genotype being: APOE ε2/ε3 (14% vs 12%), ε2/ε4 (4% vs 2%), ε3/ε4 (29% vs 22%), and ε4/ε4 (4% vs 2%) genotypes. Similar to our study, an addition of 1 e4 allele was associated with an annual faster decline on global cognition by ~ 0.05 SD, or an equivalent difference of 0.50 SD over a period of 10 years. While Rajan and colleagues found this effect size for both Whites and African Americans, our study found this effect size only among Whites, and for verbal memory rather than global mental status [43]. Unlike Rajan et al., we did not detect associations with APOE2 dosages, possibly due to the lower genotypic frequencies with ε2 alleles compared to their study or lower cognitive performance variability, thus reducing statistical power. Our study also included other potentially confounding covariates, such as a measure of literacy, which was not accounted for in the models for most past studies that tested associations between APOE genotypes and cognitive decline.

In response to the consistent finding that the ε4 allele may be detrimental to cognitive health according to previous studies (e.g., [43]), investigators have turned towards understanding resilience factors against cognitive decline among carriers of the ε4 allele. For example, in a study by Kaup and colleagues [44], the authors tested factors that predicted cognitive resilience among carriers of the ε4 allele. The authors reported variation with respect to the factors identified among white and black adults and their associated magnitudes which may be suggestive of gene-environment interactions in the associations between race, APOE, and cognitive decline. Our main findings were not affected by introducing lifestyle or health-related factors into the models. Other secondary findings that did not survive correction for multiple testing were affected by inclusion of factors such as smoking and diet quality. Future studies should uncover how these lifestyle and health-related factors play a role in the association between APOE and various neuro-cognitive outcomes.

Sex differences in the association between APOE genotypes or allele dosages with various cognitive outcomes were mostly studied among individuals of European ancestry [5, 6]. In one study by Beydoun et al., it was found that even though APOE4(+) status (i.e., any ε4 allele) predicted dementia significantly (hazard ratio [HR] = 2.89; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.93–4.33), with nonsignificant sex differences, women had significantly stronger positive associations than men between APOE4(+) status and impairment or decline on the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; delayed recall and List A total recall) and on Verbal Fluency Test-Categories [5]. In another study, female APOE4 carriers have faster rates of memory decline than their male counterparts among MCI individuals [6]. Our present study did not replicate this finding in terms of APOE4 dosage’s association with cognitive decline (e.g., in verbal memory or fluency) being stronger among White women vs. White men, and sex differences as such were not detected in the total sample. Such lack of heterogeneity by sex was found in at least one other comparable prospective cohort study [22].

Biological mechanisms

In terms of biological plausibility, APOE genotype has long been associated with cardiovascular disease, dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease [45]. More recently, researchers have reported associations between APOE genotype and neurobiological markers of aging at various stages of the life course [46]. The three alleles that comprise APOE vary with respect to their affinity for binding to serum cholesterol which, in turn, influences the extent to which individuals can metabolize dietary fat in the blood [47]. Individual carriers of the ε2 allele have lower total serum cholesterol compared to ε3/ε3, on average, whereas carriers of ε4 tend to have higher levels relative to APOE3 homozygotes [47]; an association that has been reported at younger ages as well [45]. Nevertheless, it has been also shown that, unlike ε2/ε3, the ε2/ε2 APOE genotype was linked to higher levels of serum triglycerides which may exacerbate cerebrovascular disease, when compared with ε3/ε3 [48]. In fact, recent evidence points to a strong relationship between serum triglycerides and several markers of neurodegeneration [49]. This may explain in part the adverse (though marginal) link found in our study between APOE2 allelic dosage and decline on global mental status. However, an in-depth analysis of the association between APOE genotypes with serum lipid trajectories over time may be needed, which in turn can be studied in relation to markers of neurodegeneration. The putative protective effect of APOE4 dosage among African American women on performance in the domain of attention over time requires replication and deserves further study with respect to underlying mechanisms. Specifically, the difference between African American men and women in this association may be the result of biological or hormonal sex differences, as it could be caused by socioenvironmental factors related to gender. Finally, a recent study by Morris et al. found that lower CSF concentrations of total tau and phosphorylated tau181 in African American individuals may reflect a significant race by APOE4 dosage interaction, suggesting a differential effect of this Alzheimer risk variant in African American individuals compared with white individuals [50]. Nevertheless, this study included a limited number of APOE4 carriers among African Americans in that sample [50]. Despite the lack of assessment for ethnicity in our study, our findings may have been affected if sufficient number of participants had a Latino ethnicity as well. However, Latinos is a heterogeneous ethnicity, genetically, biologically, and culturally, and thus, it is difficult to speculate as to the direction of the change in our key findings.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of our study include its large sample size allowing us to detect small effects, both cross-sectional and longitudinal, within the context of mixed-effects linear regression models with outcomes being 11 test scores spanning various domains of cognition, in a socioeconomically and racially diverse sample of middle-aged urban adults. Given the possibility of small effect being detected, some of the main findings were presented in terms of effect size, using standardized regression coefficients, in addition to showing their statistical significance. The larger sample size and adequate balance by race and sex also allowed testing for effect modification by both of these socio-demographic factors. Our analysis adjusted for important potential confounders, corrected for multiple testing, adjusted for selection bias due to missingness in outcome data using a 2-stage Heckman selection model, included multiple imputation of covariates, and formally tested effect modification by race and sex. Our results can be extrapolated to many African American and White middle-aged urban adult populations, as HANDLS is representative of 14 urban settings across the USA [35]. Despite these strengths, our study has several limitations. Those include the relatively small number of ε2 and ε4 carriers after models were stratified by race and by race × sex, particularly for ε2 alleles (< 20 participants overall, < 10 for each race group). Nevertheless, our analyses included APOE2 or APOE4 dosages as an ordinal variable (0,1,2) as opposed to studying genotypes using a common referent approach (e.g., 1 vs. 0; 2 vs. 0). This improved statistical power in our models, although it is worth noting that for some race × sex analyses, the APOE dosage may be representing a move from “0” to “1” dosage only, given the lack of availability for the “2” category. Thus, our main focus was on the stratification by race only.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we found putative adverse associations between the APOE ε4 allele dosage and cognitive decline in the memory domain among Whites, while among African American women, APOE ε4 allele dosage had a potential protective effect on the domain of attention over time. Such domain-specific inconsistencies have been reported in other studies, particularly when comparing the effect of APOE4 allele status or dosage on memory to that on other more crystallized domains of cognition (e.g., verbal fluency or attention). APOE2 dosage appeared to have less consistent associations with various domains of cognition across racial groups. Further longitudinal studies are needed to replicate our race and sex-specific findings.