Introduction

Intelligence affects all aspects of human life [1]. During the school years, some individuals show higher intelligence, attain better marks in exams, and have better prospects for further education [2, 3]. In the workplace, intelligence influences performance, efficiency, the ability to cope with difficulties, and career achievements [4]. Intelligence is also a predictor of higher quality of life and better health outcomes [5, 6]. Revealing the biological bases of individual differences in human intelligence has become a central and enduring aim of psychological and brain sciences. During the past decade, advances in genetic research have greatly promoted our understanding of intelligence [7,8,9,10]. However, further insight on its biological basis is needed.

Understanding the role of genetic characteristics and their interaction with environmental factors is the key to reveal the biological mechanisms underlying differences in human intelligence [11]. Currently, omics technologies (such as genomics, metabolomics, etc.) are widely used to provide a comprehensive characterization at the molecular level of the human body as a biological system. These approaches have successfully identified a number of informative biomarkers and greatly advanced our knowledge of the molecular mechanisms responsible for many traits. However, most omics studies focus only on a single layer, and therefore fail to capture information across multiple omics assays [12]. Recently, researchers have linked metabolomics traits to genomic information through genome-wide association studies (GWAS) on non-targeted metabolic profiling [13,14,15]. A large database of genetically determined metabotypes (GDMs) has been thus established to provide comprehensive insights of how genetic variation influences metabolism [16]. The established GDMs provide important intermediates to reveal the role of the interactions between genetics and metabolic traits in determining differences in human intelligence.

Mendelian randomization (MR) is a novel genetic epidemiology study design using genetic variants as instrumental variables (IVs) to investigate whether a modifiable exposure is causally related to a medically relevant disease risk [17]. The fundamental assumption utilized in the MR framework is that if genetic variants essentially affect the biological effects of a modifiable exposure, they should be also related to the exposure-related disease risk. Exploiting the fact that inherent genetic variants are not generally susceptible to environmental variables, the MR design can avoid the potential confounding factors that are common in conventional observational studies [18]. In recent years, the explosion in the number of published GWAS summary data has increased the popularity of MR approaches (and in particular of two-sample MR analysis) as tools to infer the causality of risk factors on complex health outcomes [19,20,21]. In this study, using GDMs and the results of GWAS on intelligence, we implement two-sample MR analysis to: (1) assess the causal effects of genetically determined metabolites on human intelligence; (2) investigate the genetic basis that may play a central role in determining the variation of the related metabolites and the differences in human intelligence; (3) identify potential metabolic pathways involved in the biological processes related to intelligence.

Methods

GWAS scans with metabolomics traits

Shin et al. reported the most comprehensive exploration of genetic influences on human metabolism so far, by performing a GWAS of non-targeted metabolomics on 7824 healthy adults. [16]. Metabolic profiling was carried out on fasting serum using high-performance liquid chromatography and gas chromatography separation coupled with tandem mass spectrometry. After quality control, 486 metabolites were retained for genetic analysis, among which 309 were chemically identified and could be further assigned to 8 metabolic groups (amino acids, carbohydrates, cofactors and vitamins, energy, lipids, nucleotides, peptides, and xenobiotics), while the other 177 were classified as ‘unknown’. The final genome-wide association analyses were carried out on approximately 2.1 million single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). Full summary statistics for the 486 metabolites can be found at the Metabolomics GWAS Server (http://metabolomics.helmholtz-muenchen.de/gwas/).

IVs for the 486 metabolites

The foundational principle of MR relies on the existence of valid IVs. A genetic variant is a valid IV if it is (i) significantly associated with the exposure, (ii) independent of confounders, and (iii) associated with the outcome only through the exposure [22]. To identify valid IVs, we first selected the SNPs with significance P < 1 × 10−5, so as to account for a proportion as large as possible of the variance explained for the corresponding metabolite. We next performed a clumping procedure (linkage disequilibrium threshold of r2 < 0.1 within a 500-kb window) to select the independent SNPs using the PLINK software (v1.9). To avoid the negative impact of weak IVs, we further used the proportion of variation explained by each IV (R2) and the F statistics to select SNPs strong enough to be valid IVs. Typically, an F statistic > 10 is considered sufficient for MR analysis [23].

GWAS summary data on intelligence

GWAS summary statistics for intelligence were obtained from the study by Savage et al. [10]. Briefly, these authors performed a large GWAS meta-analysis of 269,867 individuals from 14 cohorts of European ancestry. Intelligence was assessed using different neurocognitive tests and the general factor of intelligence (Spearman’s g). Although differences in assessment methods might reduce the power to detect associations in meta-analyses, this approach can at the same time reduce type I errors by removing measurement errors, and therefore identify SNPs with robust associations to the common latent factor underlying intelligence across different methods. Stringent quality control procedures were applied to the summary statistics for each cohort. Association analysis was conducted controlling for covariates of age, sex, genotyping array, socioeconomic status for specific cohort, and twenty European-based ancestry principal components. Finally, a total of 9,295,118 SNPs were included in the meta-analysis.

Statistical analysis

Primary two-sample MR analyses were performed using the standard inverse-variance weighted (IVW) method. The IVW method provides a consistent estimate of causal effects by combining the ratio estimates of each variant in a fixed-effect meta-analysis model [23]. The P-value was calculated with a standard normal cumulative distribution function on the ratio of the combined causal effect and its standard error. The significance threshold to declare a causal relationship for the IVW-based MR estimate was set, using Bonferroni correction, at P < 1.03 × 10–4 (= 0.05/486). Associations with P < 0.05, but not reaching the Bonferroni-corrected threshold, were reported as suggestive of association.

The IVW method provides an unbiased estimate under the assumption that all genetic variants are valid IVs. However, this assumption is easily violated, leading to inaccurate estimates, when horizontal pleiotropy occurs (some variants act on the outcome via a different intermediary) [24]. To avoid the effects of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in MR, we further performed sensitivity analyses using three additional MR methods: the MR-Egger method, which provides a consistent causal effect estimate, even when all genetic variants violate the assumptions defining valid IVs, under a weaker assumption (known as the InSIDE [instrument strength independent of direct effect] assumption) [24]; the weighted median method, which introduces a weighted median estimator and provides a more precise estimate than MR-Egger regression without the InSIDE assumption [25]; and the MR-PRESSO method, a newly developed approach which can identify and correct for horizontal pleiotropic outliers in MR [26]. We further used the MR-PRESSO global test as well as the intercept of the MR-Egger regression to test for pleiotropy, and we also evaluated heterogeneity with the I2 and the Cochran Q test. Typically, I2 > 25% or Cochran Q-derived P < 0.05 were used as indicators of possible horizontal pleiotropy. Analyses were carried out using the packages MendelianRandomization and MR-PRESSO in R (version 3.6.1).

Replication

We next used GWAS datasets of four other related outcomes to replicate the findings of our MR estimates. The first dataset was obtained from another GWAS of intelligence with 248,482 samples from the UK Biobank [27]. Summary statistics of cognitive performance (n = 257,828) and educational attainment (n = 766,345) were obtained from the study of Lee et al. [28]. Genetic associations with income (n = 286,301) were extracted from the large publicly available Lothian Birth Cohorts of 1921 and 1936 data-sharing resource [29]. Notably, the Davies et al. reported another GWAS for intelligence with a larger sample size, but the summary data for full dataset is not available due to data permissions [30].

Metabolic pathway analysis

Metabolic pathway analysis was carried out using the web-based tool suite MetaboAnalyst 4.0 (https://www.metaboanalyst.ca/) [31]. For this analysis, we extracted all metabolites showing suggestive associations in the IVW estimates (PIVW < 0.05). Two libraries of metabolic pathways or metabolite sets were selected for enrichment analysis, namely the Small Molecule Pathway Database (SMPDB, http://www.smpdb.ca) [32] and the Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG, https://www.kegg.jp/) database [33]. P-values < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Causal effects of the metabolites on intelligence

We selected 3–675 independent genetic variants as IVs for each of the 486 metabolites (Additional file 4: Table S1). On average, the IVs explained 4.7% (range 0.8–83.5%) of the variance of their respective metabolic traits. The minimum F statistic used to evaluate the strength of these IVs was 20.33. Using these IVs, IVW identified 16 known metabolites and 16 unknown metabolites that might have causal effects on human intelligence (Fig. 1, Additional file 4: Table S2). Among the 16 known metabolic traits, 5-oxoproline was significantly associated with intelligence after Bonferroni correction (PIVW = 9.25 × 10–5). Using 25 SNPs as proxy, we observed a 0.24 increase in the score of the Spearman’s g test for an increase of one standard deviation (SD) in the level of 5-oxoproline (β = 2.10; 95% Confidence interval [CI] 0.12 to 0.35). We also found 15 other metabolites to be suggestive for association, including indolelactate (β = − 0.09; 95% CI − 0.81 to − 0.01, PIVW = 0.0313), mannitol (β = − 0.03; 95% CI − 0.06 to − 0.01, PIVW = 0.0223), and 2-oleoylglycerophosphocholine (β = 0.18; 95% CI 0.05 to 0.30, PIVW = 0.0055).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mendelian randomization associations of genetically determined metabolites on intelligence

Sensitivity analysis

Table 1 shows the results of the sensitivity analyses for the 16 IVW-identified known metabolites. The causal relationship between 5-oxoproline and intelligence was robust when additional MR methods were applied (PMR-Egger = 0.0001, PWeighted median = 6.29 × 10–6, PMR-PRESSO = 0.0007), and no horizontal pleiotropy was observed (PIntercept = 0.09, PGlobal test = 0.06, I2 = 25%, PHeterogeneity = 0.13). Two other metabolites also showed robust associations with intelligence, namely dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) (PMR-Egger = 0.0494, PWeighted median = 0.0236, PMR-PRESSO = 0.0293, PGlobal test = 0.16) and p-acetamidophenylglucuronide (PMR-Egger = 0.0075, PWeighted median = 0.0060, PMR-PRESSO = 0.0454, PGlobal test = 0.0611), and there were no evidence of horizontal pleiotropy (PIntercept = 0.24, PGlobal test = 0.17, I2 = 0%, PHeterogeneity = 0.96 for dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) and PIntercept =0.06, PGlobal test = 0.06, I2 = 17%, PHeterogeneity = 0.13 for p-acetamidophenylglucuronide; Table 1). Funnel plots appeared generally symmetrical for all the three metabolites, also suggesting no evidence for horizontal pleiotropy (Additional file 1: Fig. S1). Dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) showed a negative association with intelligence (βIVW = − 0.14; 95% CI − 0.25 to − 0.04), while the association between p-acetamidophenylglucuronide and intelligence was positive (βIVW = 0.01; 95% CI 0.00 to 0.01). The causal association between 5-oxoproline and human intelligence is shown on Fig. 2, while the associations for dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) and p-acetamidophenylglucuronide with intelligence are represented on Fig. 3. Notably, the very small effect size for p-acetamidophenylglucuronide on intelligence might limit its potential utility as a biomarker.

Table 1 Sensitivity analysis of causal associations between metabolites and intelligence
Fig. 2
figure 2

Genetic associations between 5-oxoproline and intelligence

Fig. 3
figure 3

Genetic associations of two suggestive metabolites with intelligence. a Dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6); b p-acetamidophenylglucuronide

Associations with other relevant outcomes

We next repeated the main findings using summary statistics from other data sources. Figure 4 showed the results of causal effects of 5-oxoproline on human intelligence from another data source, cognitive performance, educational attainment, and income. The effect of genetically determined 5-oxoproline on intelligence (Replication) was similar (β = 0.17; 95% CI 0.04 to 0.30, PIVW = 0.0087) to the result of initial MR estimates, and the causal associations were robust when different methods were performed (PWeighted median = 0.0003, PMR-Egger = 0.0035). The results also showed that 5-oxoproline was significantly associated with cognitive performance (PIVW = 0.0001, PWeighted median = 1.44 × 10–6, PMR-Egger = 0.0009). However, no evidences for association were found between 5-oxoproline and educational attainment (PIVW = 0.5595, PWeighted median = 0.3417, PMR-Egger = 0.4611), as well as income (PIVW = 0.7854, PWeighted median = 0.4287, PMR-Egger = 0.6178). Besides, the effects of dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) and p-acetamidophenylglucuronide on intelligence were also significant in the replication stage (Additional file 2: Fig. S2; Additional file 3: Fig. S3).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mendelian randomization associations of 5-oxoproline on other intelligence-related outcomes from other data sources

Genetic basis for the causal associations

We further investigated the genetic variants that affected both metabolite levels and intelligence. Table 2 shows the 25 SNPs used as IV of 5-oxoproline. Among them, rs11986602 showed the most significant association with 5-oxoproline (β = − 0.0620; SE = 0.0029, P = 6.29 × 10–104). Notably, it also showed a strong association signal with intelligence (β = − 0.0196; SE = 0.0044, P = 9.53 × 10–6). Moreover, this SNP had the largest effect sizes on both 5-oxoproline and intelligence, suggesting that the related genetic locus might provide valuable information on the biological mechanisms of intelligence, and that 5-oxoproline might be an important functional intermediate to understand the biological process through which genetics affects intelligence. The IVs for dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) and p-acetamidophenylglucuronide are shown in Additional file 4: Tables S3 and S4.

Table 2 Genetic predictors of 5-oxoproline and their association with Intelligence

Metabolic pathway analysis

Table 3 shows the results of the metabolic pathway analysis. Based on the 16 known metabolites identified by the IVW method, we detected only one significant metabolic pathway associated with intelligence, namely Alpha linolenic acid and linoleic acid metabolism (P = 0.0062). Two metabolites identified by IVW, docosapentaenoate (n3 DPA; 22:5n3) and linolenate (18:3n3 or 6), are involved in Alpha linolenic acid and linoleic acid metabolism according to the SMPDB database. Importantly, many of the metabolites found by our analysis have not been assigned to any metabolic pathway currently recorded in the SMPDB or KEGG databases. Extensive further research will be needed to explore whether these metabolites are involved in biological processes relevant to differences in human intelligence.

Table 3 Results of metabolic pathway analysis

Discussion

We implemented a two-sample MR analysis to assess the causal relationships between genetically determined metabolites and human intelligence. Using genetic variants as IVs, we found that the genetically determined levels of 5-oxoproline were associated with better performance in human intelligence tests. This causal association was not affected by confounders such as educational attainment and household income, and was well replicated using samples from other data source. Our study also identified other metabolites and metabolic pathways involved in biological processes related to human intelligence, such as dihomo-linoleate (20:2n6) and p-acetamidophenylglucuronide. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study combining information from genomics and metabolomics to assess the causal effects of metabolome traits on human intelligence.

5-Oxoproline, also known as pyroglutamic acid, is a cyclized derivative of l-glutamic acid that participates substantially in the glutamate and glutathione metabolism [34]. Disturbances in glutamate and glutathione metabolism can lead to a series of neurologic phenotypes, including developmental delay, ataxia, seizures, and intellectual disability [35]. Moreover, 5-oxoproline was also developed and sold as an over-the-counter “smart drug” for cognitive and memory improvement [36, 37]. However, it was also demonstrated that metabolic acidosis could be caused by excessive 5-oxoproline generation, with multiple adverse effects on many organ systems [38]. Our study found that elevated levels of 5-oxoproline were associated with a higher score in intelligence tests, supporting the potential usefulness of 5-oxoproline in improving intelligence-related performance. However, more work aimed at understanding the molecular mechanisms involved is needed to further clarify the role of this compound in human intelligence.

Genetic factors played a central role in our study of the causal relationship between metabolic traits and intelligence. The SNP rs11986602 (corresponding to the EXOSC4 gene) was the most significantly associated to both 5-oxoproline levels and human intelligence. Although rarely discussed in the past literature, EXOSC4 is known to be related to the protein kinase R (PKR)-like endoplasmic reticulum kinase (PERK, encoded by the EIF2AK3 gene), which regulates gene expression [39]. A recent study reported that locally reduced PERK expression or activity could enhance neuronal excitability and improve memory and cognitive function in young mice [40]. Another study provided evidence that PERK is a key regulator of memory impairments and neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s disease [41]. Thus, EXOSC4 might be a causal risk gene participating in physiological processes important for human intelligence.

We further focused on the metabolic pathways that might be involved in the biological processes associated to human intelligence. The only identified metabolic pathway in our study was Alpha linolenic acid and linoleic acid metabolism. Alpha linolenic acid and linoleic acid are long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, which are essential nutrients in the development and functioning of the brain [42]. Many related compounds, such as alpha linolenic acid and docosahexaenoic acid, are involved in the rapid growth and development of the infant brain [43, 44]. Our study thus reinforced the importance of alpha linolenic acid and linoleic acid metabolism for human intelligence, providing valuable information for understanding the biological mechanisms related to human intelligence.

The current study has several strengths. First, we implemented a novel MR study design to assess the causal relationships between genetically determined metabolites and human intelligence. By using genetic variants as IVs, the MR approach prevents confounding, reverse causation, and various biases common in observational epidemiological studies. Second, our study provides, indirectly, a comprehensive assessment of the causal effects of metabolites assessed by non-targeted metabolomics on human intelligence. Third, by integrating genomics and metabolomics, our study provides novel insight into the biological mechanisms underlying differences in intelligence.

There are also several limitations that should be noted. First, the GWAS data for intelligence was determined adjusting for socioeconomic status, which was a heritable and correlated secondary trait to intelligence [29, 45]. The adjustment for socioeconomic status might cause bias in genetic associations with intelligence for some SNPs [46]. Second, our study could not avoid the bias of dynastic effect, which induced a correlation between the environment a child is raised in and their genetic inheritance and almost certainly violated the independence assumption of MR [47, 48]. Within family GWAS data was useful in avoiding the issue of dynastic effects. However, such data was not available at this stage. Third, our study failed to perform the bi-directional MR analysis which was useful in detecting false positive MR results arising from genetic correlation between traits. The reason was that many of the IVs for intelligence were missing in datasets of metabolites. Finally, the MR estimates from non-experimental date could not provide information towards molecular mechanism, further work should be done to determine the roles of metabolites or genetic variants in development of intelligence.

In summary, our study identified multiple metabolites that might have causal effects on human intelligence, among which 5-oxoproline presented significant association signals after Bonferroni correction. The association was shown to be robust by sensitivity analyses. Our study also highlighted that genetic factors (e.g. the EXOSC4 gene) contributed substantially to the variation of metabolite levels and differences in human intelligence. Moreover, our findings suggest that alpha linolenic acid and linoleic acid metabolism might be involved in the biological processes underlying intelligence. Though further evidence from experimental data is needed, our study provides novel clues that would improve our understanding of the biological mechanisms related to human intelligence.