Background

Preschool-age children, generally aged between three and five, spend about half of their waking hours in sedentary [1] . Sedentary time (ST) is defined as the time spent in sedentary behaviors (SB); SB is defined as any waking behavior characterized by an energy expenditure of less than or equal to 1.5 metabolic equivalents (METs) while in a sitting, reclining or lying posture [2]. ST can therefore consist of multiple different types of SBs (e.g. screen time, playing still and reading) that can accumulate throughout the week and across multiple settings. Higher ST in adulthood is associated with negative health consequences such as chronic disease outcomes and all-cause mortality, although high levels of daily physical activity (PA) may eliminate these risks [3,4,5]. The evidence of health consequences of overall ST in preschool-age is less conclusive [6]. However, as many other health behavior habits, SB habits are rooted in early childhood and tend to track later in life [7]. As preschool children spend significant proportions of their time in sedentary, it is relevant to study the factors associated with children’s ST [1, 8, 9]. For instance, studies have identified the role of home and preschool settings in influencing preschool children’s ST, stating that children in center-based care such as preschool are highly sedentary compared to children cared for at home; however, the opposite results have also been found [1, 10]. The difference in these results may relate to different ST measurement methods or cultural context of studies. This dissimilarity also underlines the importance of understanding which factors may cause variations in preschool children’s ST.

According to the socioecological models of health behaviors [11], multiple factors at a variety of ecological levels can influence ST. These correlates may be positioned at individual levels: for example, biological and temperament characteristics; or at environmental settings:such as home and preschool [11,12,13]. The environmental setting can consist of the physical environment (the type of available physical environment), social environment (the type of attitudes and beliefs of significant others) and, regarding preschool, also the organizational environment (the type of rules and regulations) [14]. Due to the early developmental stage of preschool children, children are highly receptive and dependent on their caregivers in their daily movement behaviors (e.g. ST, and PA) [15]. Caregivers in both of these settings, such as guardians, principals and early educators, can help to shape children’s possibilities to conduct movement behaviors through a variety of social environmental mechanisms, including encouragement, beliefs and attitudes, role modelling, rules and regulations, involvement and facilitation [16, 17]. Thus, it is important to understand which social environmental factors in both settings are associated with children’s ST.

Although the number of studies regarding correlates of preschool children’s ST has risen following Hinkleys’ et al review in 2010 [18], insufficient evidence prevents drawing conclusions about the correlates of ST. For instance, numerous studies have recognized that girls are more sedentary than boys. However, the influence of other child characteristics such as temperament on preschool children’s ST is less studied [1, 19]. Similarly, a recent review highlights the lack of knowledge about preschool social- and organizational level factors associated with children’s ST [20]. Slightly more knowledge exists of associations between social environment in the home setting and preschool children’s screen time as an indicator of ST [21]; however, the correlates of screen time and overall ST seem to be distinct [22]. Another relevant aspect of social environment is socioeconomic status (SES) and its associations with children’s behaviors [23]. Studies indicate associations between lower parental SES or lower neighborhood SES, and a higher risk of an unhealthier lifestyle and cardiovascular disease [24,25,26,27]. However, studies regarding the associations between parental or neighborhood SES and children’s ST are scarce, with contradictory findings, which can result from the variety of possible ways to measure SES [24, 28].

Many of these previous studies have also explored the influence of either preschool OR home; the influence of the combination of these settings is lacking. Following the principles of socioecological models, these settings interact with each other influencing children’s behavior [14]. Thus, a recent systematic review encourages the exploration of potential correlates across multiple levels of influence of socioecological model within one study [29]. Based on these above-mentioned considerations, this study aims to investigate which individual-, home- and preschool-level factors are associated with preschool children’s ST. In addition, this study aims to discover the extent to which individual-, home- and preschool-level factors explain variance in children’s ST.

Methods

Study design and population

The cross-sectional DAGIS (Increased Health and Wellbeing in Preschools) survey was conducted in eight municipalities in Southern and Western Finland in 2015 and 2016. More detailed information about the study purposes and the flow chart of participation in this study can be read elsewhere [30, 31]. Briefly, the main recruitment criterion for the randomly invited preschools was the existence of at least one preschool group with children aged between three and six. In the chosen municipalities, 86 preschools (51% of those invited) agreed to participate in the study, and organized the recruitment of families and children. A total of 983 guardians (27% of contacted guardians) gave written permission for their child to participate in the study. Implementation of study procedures required participation of at least 30% of children in at least one preschool group in a preschool; 91 guardians had a child in a preschool with less than the 30% participation rate and were thus excluded from the study. In addition, 28 children did not have any data. A total of 864 (24% of invited) children from 66 preschools (39% of invited preschools) participated in the study. The University of Helsinki Ethical Review Board in the Humanities and Social and Behavioral Sciences approved the study procedures (6/2015, approved on 25th February, 2015).

Measures

Children’s sedentary time

Children wore an Actigraph W-GT3X accelerometers (Actigraph, LLC, Fort Walton Beach, Florida) for one week. Actigraphs have been validated and used extensively as an objective measure of PA and ST for different age groups and in different contexts [32,33,34]. Research group set the accelerometers on the children’s right hip during the first day of measurement in the preschool. After data collection, the epoch length was set at 15 s. Periods of 10 min or more at zero accelerometer counts were considered non-wear times and were excluded [35]; however, possible nap-times were not excluded. The analyses applied the Evenson ST cut-point (0–25 counts per 15 s) [36], a renonwned estimate of free-living ST [37, 38]. We set the following accelerometer wear-time criteria for children in the analyses: at least 600 min of wear time each day; at least four days with at least one weekend day during a measurement week. Because wearing hours varied between children, the variable was adjusted for the wearing hours so that the overall ST minutes was divided by the the total accelerometer wearing time and multiplied by 60 to create outcome variable expressed as average minutes per hour (min/h). Consequently, the measure used in this study indicates the average children’s ST minutes in one hour.

Individual level

Research group measured the height, weight and waist circumference of participating children in the preschool. These measurements allowed calculation of children’s body mass index (BMI) by dividing the weight in kilograms by the square of the height in meters. Each guardian reported their child’s age and gender in the guardian’s questionnaire. Guardians were given a separate questionnaire regarding children’s temperament, which asked guardians to consider their children’s reactions to 36 situations in the past six months. The questionnaire is based on a short form of the Child Behavior Questionnaire for children aged three to seven [39], which has demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency, criterion validity and longitudinal stability and provides a comprehensive assessment of children’s temperament [39]. The separate 36 items are typically coded into three summary scores to illustrate separate temperament domains: negative affectivity, effortful control and surgency. A child with a high level of negative affectivity is typically angry and fearful with a lowered mood and low soothability. A child with a high level of effortful control enjoys low-intensity activities and is characterized by increased attentional capacities, inhibitory control. A child with high surgency levels is impulsive, enjoys situations with high stimulus intensity and feels comfortable in social situations [39].

Home level

One guardian in each family completed the guardian’s questionnaire that covers attitudes, beliefs, self-efficacy, practices and availability of items related to children’s energy balance-related behaviors (EBRBs). This questionnaire also had multiple questions about family SES. Guardians could complete the questionnaire either online or on paper. The questions were based on formative work of the research group, the previously validated questions and items specific to the Finnish context [40,41,42,43]. The questionnaire included items related either to screen time, ST or PA because, as yet, only a limited number of associations between ST and potential correlates among preschool children have been recognized. In addition, we chose a mothers’ education as the indicator of parental SES because it seems to be the strongest, reliable and most consistent determinant of children’s health behaviors and childhood obesity [24, 44, 45]. Table 1 presents all the items used in this study. The selection of these home setting measures for this study was based on the following criterias: a) the included measures capture social environment related to children’s ST extensively, b) the measures are measured similarly (or almost similarly) in preschool level, and c) the measures are relevant to measure based on the previous research findings.

Table 1 Summary of measures included in the analysis

Preschool level

Multiple questionnaires were used to measure the preschool setting. Principals were asked to complete a web-based questionnaire covering both the rules and regulations related to EBRBs in their preschool and their own attitudes and beliefs about children’s EBRBs. Because each preschool group in a preschool can define their own practices, we had an additional questionnaire for each preschool group that more precisely measured their group-based practices related to children’s EBRBs. One early educator in each preschool group completed this paper-based questionnaire. The questions in both questionnaires were based on formative work of the research group, the previously validated questions and items developed suitable for the Finnish context [46,47,48]. The preschool neighborhood SES was obtained from Statistics Finland. This grid database contains coordinate-based statistical data calculated on a map grid within a one-kilometer radius from the participating preschools. Table 1 presents all the items used in this study. The selection of these home setting measures for this study was based on the following criterias: a) the included measures capture social or organisational environment related to children’s ST extensively, b) the measures are measured similarly (or almost similarly) in home level, and c) the measures are relevant to measure based on the previous research findings.

Covariates

Covariates in the analyses were ‘Children’s average attendance at preschool’ and ‘Study season’. ‘Children’s attendance at preschool’ was a composite score of the following questions in the guardians’ questionnaires: ‘How many days per week does your child attend preschool? ’ and ‘How many hours per day does your child usually attend preschool? ’. The average children’s daily attendance hours in preschool (hours/day) was formed by combining these measures. The ‘Study season’ measure was divided into three categories: 1 = September–October, 2 = November–December, and 3 = January–April.

Statistical analyses

The descriptive statistics, factor analysis of sum variables and multicollinearity tests were conducted using the SPSS statistical program version 24 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Multicollinearity was tested using tolerance and variance inflation factors. The results indicated no issues with multicollinearity.

Linear regression models examined the associations between explanatory factors and children’s ST, with analyses conducted in multiple steps. First, the main effects of all the potential explanatory factors was individually examined. These regression models were conducted in Mplus version 7.14. (Muthen & Muthen, 2017). MLR (maximum likelihood with robust standard errors) was used as an estimator in the analyses. These models accounted for clustering of children within families with participating siblings, preschools and preschool groups. If the responses were nested in the same higher-level unit, such as children attending the same preschool, multilevel design were applied. In these multilevel models, children were set as the first level unit, and the preschools or preschool groups as the second level unit. In addition, each individual level variable was group-mean centered [49, 50].

In the second phase, all the explanatory factors indicating association with ST (P-value < 0.10) from the previous phase were included in the same model. For this model, we calculated the marginal and the conditional R2 as per Nakawaga and Schilzeth (2013). Marginal R2 is the variance explained by fixed factors in the model, and conditional R2 is the variance explained by fixed and random factors [51]. After estimating the model, the “sum square” of each fixed factor (independent variables) was used to calculate the explained variance for each independent variable. First, the percentage of explained variance between all fixed factors was calculated. As the fixed factors explained only 29.8% of the variance, variance explained was estimated to the full model.

Results

Of the 864 participating children, 773 children had valid accelerometer data for the analyses. Of them, 48% were girls. The average age of children with accelerometer data was 4 years and 7 months (SD = 0.86).On average, the children had 28.1 min of ST per hour (SD = 4 min/h). A total of 809 guardians completed the guardian’s questionnaire. Of them, 88% were mothers and 57% of respondents completed the questionnaire online (N = 461). Table 1 presents the mean and standard deviation for continuous explanatory variables and percentages for categorical explanatory variables.

Table 2 presents the results of the first models. All the 29 potential explanatory factors were included individually in the analyses. These first models indicated that being a boy (p = 0.000) and having high surgency temperament characteristics (p = 0.000) were associated with lower ST, whereas having high levels of effortful control was associated with higher ST(p = 0.000). Regarding the home setting, the following factors were associated with children’s lower ST: parental self-efficacy for activating child for PA (p = 0.036) and frequent visits together in PA places (p = 0.002). Parental perceived barriers related to children’s outside PA (p = 0.001) was associated with children’s higher ST.

Table 2 Associations of each explanatory factor and children’s sedentary time (min/h) in multilevel linear regression models

Regarding the preschool setting, the following factors were associated with children’s lower ST: more frequent visits in PA places (p = 0.047) and written policies about guidance for families (p = 0.039). Preschool principals’ personal interest in health (p = 0.001 in the comparison between strongly agree and neither agree or disagree) and principals’ beliefs of unhealthy EBRBs as a problem (p = 0.021) were associated with children’s higher ST.

Table 3 presents the results of the final models. Analysis of these models included the explanatory factors that showed at least some evidence of an association with the previous phase (total 11 factors). Of the individual level factors, being a boy (p < 0.001) and having high levels of surgency-type temperament (p < 0.001) was associated with children’s lower ST. Regarding the home setting, more frequent visits together in PA places (p = 0.014) was associated with children’s lower ST, whereas parental attitude about barriers related to children’s physical activity outside (p = 0.032) was associated with children’s higher ST. None of the preschool-level factors remained significant.

Table 3 Final models: associations of explanatory factors and children’s sedentary time (min/h) in multilevel linear regression models

Fixed effects in the final model explained 29.8% of the variance (marginal R2) in children’s ST. The proportion of variance explained in the model, including all fixed effects plus the random effect, was 53.5%, which indicates that the random factor (preschool) caused much of the total variance. Table 4 shows the 29.8% of marginal variance separately for individual-, home- and preschool-levels. Approximately 11% of the variance in children’s ST was attributed to factors related to the individual level; approximately 5% was attributed to home-level factors; and approximately 2% was attributed to preschool-level factors. Of the additional fixed factors, approximately 12% was attributed to the study season, and about 0.3% was attributed to the children’s average attendance at preschool.

Table 4 Percentage of variance explained by variance between fixed factors or by variance in full model

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine individual-, home- and preschool level- factors associated with preschool children’s ST. Additionally, it was studied how much individual-, home- and preschool- level factors explained the variance in children’s ST. Of the individual level factors, being a boy and having a surgency-type temperament was associated with lower ST. Of the home-level factors, parental attitude about barriers related to children’s PA outside was associated with children’s higher ST, whereas more frequent visits together in PA places was associated with children’s lower ST. None of the measured preschool-level factors remained significant in the final models. Together, these results suggest that individual-level factors explain more the variance in children’s ST than home and preschool settings. Notably, the proportion of variance explained in ST, which included all fixed effects plus the random effect, was approximately 54%. This high percentage indicates that the random factor (preschool) captured substantial amounts of additional variance. However, the variance of preschool factors used in this study remained low. This difference indicates that we may not have measured the relevant preschool factors in our study to fully explain the potential variance in children’s ST.

Two individual-level factors, namely being a boy and having high levels of surgency, were associated with lower ST. The individual-level factors also explained most of the variance in children’s ST. Our findings support the results of many other studies: boys have less ST than girls [1]. Two recent studies concluded that activity temperament was associated with lower ST [52] whereas anxious temperament was associated with higher ST [53]. The studies also indicated that mothers with low income or who are overweight particularly dealt with children’s challenging temperament traits by allowing their children use the television [54, 55]. In addition, Gubbels et al [14, 53] recognized that gender or temperament moderated the association between a preschool setting and children’s movement behaviors. These previous studies have used different temperament instrument than this study. However, similar to our findings, these results underline that children’s temperament influences their willingness to engage in movement behaviors already very early in life. Consequently, an improved strategy may involve better recognizing children’s individual needs and developing intervention strategies that are tailored to their specific characteristics.

To our knowledge, no previous studies have included factors in both preschool and home setting in the same study as we did, and therefore making comparisons to previous studies is difficult. However, Schmutz et al stated that environmental factors, for example, children living in a dual-guardian household, concerns of neighborhood safety, apartment size and guardians with no sports-club membership explained approximately 17% of the variance in children’s ST [52]. On the contrary, Hnatiuk et al found no other home-level factor associated with three-to-four-year-old children’s ST, except a father’s television viewing before 6 pm [56]. However, no preschool-level factors were included in these studies. Nonetheless, Hesketh et al stated that British preschool explained little variance in children’s anthropometry [57], and other studies had stated that a preschool setting explained 10 to 50% variance in children’s PA [58,59,60,61]. However, less is known about variance in children’s ST explained by preschool setting. Preschool care in Finland is widely regulated and standardized, ensuring similar levels of care for all children. Therefore, the preschool setting possibly has little influence in explaining the variance in children’s ST. In addition, each setting may differ if ST is a personal choice or ST has environmental and social constraints [62]. Preschool setting may have multiple social norms for sitting and expectations for children to sit, for instance, morning circles, regular meal times or puzzle sessions [42], which are quite similar across preschools. Thus, few opportunities may exist for children’s individual choice, causing little variance in children’s ST.

Most of the factors in preschool settings were measured from the perspective of principals, but how well these items capture the daily practices conducted in preschool is questionable. For instance, various policies were measured from the perspective of principals; however, how aware early educators are of these policies is unknown, along with how much these policies are actually implemented in daily practices. It may therefore be that the questionnaire for principals captured inadequately the variables that were aimed to measure due to the difficulty in operationalizing the terms of attitudes, beliefs and policies. Overall, our results support the need to study other potential factors associated with children’s ST in the preschool setting. In addition, of all the factors included in this study, study season had the highest percentage of variance. Evidence indicates seasonal variation in children’s ST and PA, that is, children are more sedentary in rainy and dark weather during autumn and winter time [63,64,65]. This data was collected between early autumn and early spring, covering a wide variety of weather conditions.

Another research finding related to this theme was the association between parental perceived barriers related to children’s outside PA and children’s higher ST. The items included in this sum variable related to poor weather or difficulty of supervising a child outside. Previous qualitative studies have also highlighted parental attitudes on how poor weather, namely cold or rainy weather, limits children’s PA and how a child is not allowed to play outside alone for safety reasons [66]. We did not measure in detail what type of weather parents consider is poor weather; however, the development of intervention strategies to account for these parental concerns regarding the weather or outside supervision is important as these concerns may limit children’s possibilities to be active.

One study finding concerned the association between having frequent practices of visiting PA places with children in the home setting and lower children’s ST. Similarly, frequent visits in PA places in the preschool setting was associated with lower children’s ST in the individual models, although this association did not remain statistically significant in the final models. Overall, this practice of frequent visits to PA places may be a good target when developing intervention strategies aiming to reduce children’s ST. Being active in PA places may provide better possibilities for children to have unstructured free play that allows them to move around, and therefore is associated with lower ST. Regular co-participation in PA, either inside or outside, benefits the development of children’s social, self-regulation and motor skills, but also naturally provides healthy role-modelling opportunities to caregivers [67]. Most of the items included in these visiting PA places variables were activities conducted outside, for instance, in yards, playgrounds and nature. Previous studies have identified that time outdoors is associated with lower children’s ST [68, 69]. This study did not find any significant associations between SES factors and children’s ST, which supports the findings of other studies that measured ST by objective measurement [24]. However, mediators (intervening factors) may exist between the associations of SES and children’s ST. Conducting mediational analyses, despite non-significant overall associations, may enable an easier understanding of the mechanisms of influence [70]. In addition, some SES differences may exist in the presented co-participation practices. For instance, the previous study using this same study sample [71] found out that guardians with a low educational background reported more frequent visits with their child to their own yard, whereas guardians with high educational backgrounds reported more frequent visits with their children to indoor sports facilities. However, only frequent co-participation in own yard acted as mediator in the associations between guardians with a low educational background and children’s ST. [71]

The current study has several strengths and limitations that should be accounted for. Multiple items that were similarly asked from the perspective of both guardians and preschools were measured. However, some of the measured items were not validated, and had quite low Cronbach alpha. In addition, other important factors that influence children’s ST may exist, such as early educators’ motivation and self-efficacy. For instance, previous study using this same study sample found out that early educators’ practice often interrupting children’s ST, more frequent physical activity (PA) theme weeks, and higher number of physical education (PE) lessons were associated with children’s lower ST in preschools [72]. Our study did not cover physical environment factors either in home or preschool settings, which have most often been associated with children’s ST at least in preschool setting [20]. However, multiple factors that are rarely measured were included. Compared to other studies among preschool-aged children, the sample size of this study was quite high and covered a variety of preschools around Finland. While a hip-placed accelerometer can provide ST over a prolonged period, they are less valid in distinguishing sedentary postures, such as lying and sitting, from other light-intensity activities performed while standing. Different definitions of ST, different used cut-points and different data reduction methods may explain some of the dissimilarity between our results and previous studies. In addition, it may be relevant to measure children’s ST with multiple methods. Proxy-reports and observations may provide valuable additional knowledge of the specific SBs (e.g. reading, screen time) that cannot be captured with accelerometers. This knowledge is important as the correlates of specific SBs seem to be different than that of overall ST [22]. Our data is cross-sectional, which prevents drawing causal inferences. The detected associations between explanatory factors and outcome were relatively small. It may be that it was failed to find associations when one existed due to a lack of power and sample size. However, by including multiple levels of influence, especially using both home and preschool settings within the same study, our study introduced novel information about factors explaining the variance in preschool children’s ST.

Conclusion

This study recognized multiple factors on individual, home and preschool levels that were associated with preschool children’s ST. Children’s individual characteristics such as gender and temperament were associated with children’s ST, and explained more the variance in children’s ST than settings.. Future strategies aiming to reduce children’s ST should consequently tailor intervention strategies to children’s individual characteristics, without forgetting the influence of home and preschool settings. The preschool-level factors included in this study explained little the variance in children’s ST between preschools, possibly because the included measures did not capture the essential factors in preschool setting that influence children’s ST. In conclusion,the development of improved methods of measuring the preschool-level factors is essential.