Introduction

With the aging population worldwide, dementia causes a heavy burden on human society [1]. Patients with advanced stages of dementia can be severely disabled, and the prevalence of dementia is steadily increasing. However, specific drugs to treat dementia are still lacking. People with cognitive impairment likely develop dementia decades later [2]. The global prevalence of cognitive impairment increases every year and ranges from 6 to 12%. Preventing cognitive dysfunction could significantly reduce the high prevalence of dementia worldwide [3]. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment among the Chinese older population over 60 years old reached 15.5% (15.2–15.9) in 2020, with a total population of approximately 38.77 million [4].

Recent studies have linked low serum uric acid (SUA) levels with poor cognitive performance [5]. In a case–control study with random sampling, the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) score was linearly and inversely associated with SUA level [6]. Prospective cohort studies also found that a low baseline SUA level could be a risk factor for multiple domains of cognitive function in older adults [7]. SUA acts as a major antioxidant and reduces the risk of neurodegenerative diseases by protecting neurons from oxidative damages [5, 8].

Considerable evidence has shown that blood glucose affects cognitive function [9, 10]. Diabetes mellitus (DM), glycemic control, and DM duration are related to cognitive dysfunction [11]. A previous study reported that fasting plasma glucose (FPG) levels are associated with executive function, but not other cognitive domains. Therefore, high glucose levels may concur to the multifactorial pathogenesis of cognitive dysfunction. Appropriate blood glucose regulation may thus improve the protection of executive function in cognitively impaired older adults [12].

Mice lacking SUA transporters in the gut likely develop metabolic syndrome and hyperuricemia, indicating evidence of the causal connection between high levels of SUA and blood glucose [13]. Another study found that hyperinsulinemia inhibits renal SUA metabolism and leads to elevated SUA levels, suggesting an association between SUA and hypoglycemia [14]. Other studies reported no significant relationship between SUA and FPG [15]. A previous work on the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) found that men and women showed an L-shaped relationship between SUA and blood glucose [16]. Furthermore, the association of SUA and FPG with cognitive function among older adults remains unclear.

This study aimed to explore the association of FPG, SUA, and their combination with cognitive function and to predict the progression of cognitive function impairment to a certain extent.

Materials and methods

Study population

CHARLS is a full-scale, nationally representative longitudinal survey of middle-aged and older adults (≥ 45 years old) conducted by the National Institute of Development at Peking University. The survey used data from the 2011 national baseline survey with 17,705 participants. The exact content of CHARLS was detailed elsewhere [17]. Participants who were < 45 years old (n = 363), had a proxy interview (n = 1212), did not complete a cognitive assessment (n = 6468), had a history of brain damage or intellectual disability at baseline (n = 130), and had missing FPG or SUA data (n = 3023) were excluded from our study. Finally, a total of 6509 participants were included for analysis (Table 1).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics

CHARLS was approved by the Peking University Ethics Review Committee. Informed consent was sought from all participants.

Cognitive assessment

The participants were given cognitive measures (including the two cognitive areas of episodic memory and mental status) by uniformly trained investigators. The investigators read a list of Chinese words to the participants. After which, the participants immediately enumerated the nouns that they heard and then recalled as many nouns as possible after 5 min (delayed recall). Episodic memory was generally defined as the sum of immediate and delayed recall scores, ranging from 0 to 20. Mental status was assessed using the Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status (TICS) questionnaire, which is an adequate and reasonable method to capture the mental status or integrity of a person. The participants answered the following questions: subtract 7 several times in a row from 100 (up to five times); name the date of the day (day, week, month, year, and season), and redraw the picture he/she had been shown. The right answers were summed up to a single TICS score ranging from 0 to 11. The global cognition score was determined by summing up the episodic memory and TICS scores and ranged from 0 to 31.

Measurements of SUA and FPG

Blood samples were collected from the participants after they fasted overnight by uniformly trained staff of the Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention following standard protocols. FPG level was determined at the central study laboratory in Beijing. SUA level (mg/dL) was analyzed using SUA Plus method, and FPG was measured using an enzymatic colorimetric test. The participants were divided into groups based on their SUA and FPG quartiles. They were also divided into the following groups to evaluate the effect of interplay between SUA and FPG on cognitive function with SUA Q1–Q3 only (Low SUA), with FPG Q4 only (High FPG), without low SUA and high FPG levels (Non), and with low SUA and high FPG levels (Both).

Covariates

Several potential covariates were collected including age, sex, body mass index (BMI), marital status (currently married or not), educational level (primary school or lower, middle school, and high school or above), smoking status (current, former, and never), alcohol consumption (current, former, and never), diagnosis of hypertension, diabetes, dyslipidemia, kidney disease, stroke, heart diseases, liver disease, gastrointestinal disease, psychiatric problems, and treatment of kidney disease and diabetes (including taking Chinese or western traditional medicine and taking insulin injections or other treatments).

Statistical analysis

Continuous variables that were not normally distributed or had heterogeneity of variance were represented by median [interquartile range]. The remaining continuous variables were represented by means (standard deviations). Categorical variables were represented by percentages. Depending on the situation, Kruskal–Wallis test or analysis of variance was used for continuous variables. Chi-square test was used for classified variables. Multivariable linear regression models were used to examine the association between SUA and FPG levels and cognitive scores.

Subgroup analysis of sex was performed because sex was associated with lower scores on cognitive testing in the multivariate model. The statistical significance of the interactions was assessed by adding a multiplicative term to the linear regression model. Sensitivity analysis was conducted to determine the robustness of the primary results. All data were analyzed using STATA version 14 (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas, USA).

Results

Baseline characteristics

The baseline characteristics of the study population are listed in Table 1. The Non, Low SUA, High FPG, and Both groups had 1117, 3764, 511, and 1117 participants, respectively. The average age of the total population was 57 [51–64] years, and the male population accounted for 50.03%. Statistically significant differences were found in age, sex, FPG, UA, smoking status, BMI, DM, hypertension, dyslipidemia, stroke, heart diseases, gastrointestinal disease and diabetes treatment among all groups.

Association between SUA/FPG level and cognitive function

First, the association of SUA level with different cognitive domains was examined. After adjusting for age, sex, BMI, smoking status, hypertension, dyslipidemia, stroke, heart diseases, kidney disease, liver disease, gastrointestinal disease, diabetes and diabetes treatment, the participants in the lower SUA quartiles had incrementally lower cognitive scores (global cognition and episodic memory) than those in the highest quartile groups. The β values (95% CI) of the participants were -0.460 (-0.768, -0.153) for the SUA Q2 group and -0.420 (-0.731, -0.109) for SUA Q1 group in global cognition scores and -0.339 (-0.557, -0.122) for the SUA Q2 group and -0.311 (-0.531, -0.091) for the SUA Q1 group in episodic memory compared with those in the SUA Q4 group (Table 2).

Table 2 Association between quartiles of SUA and cognitive function

The association of FPG level with different cognitive domains was also examined. After adjusting for age, sex, BMI, smoking status, hypertension, dyslipidemia, stroke, heart diseases, and diabetes treatment, no statistical association was found between FPG and cognitive score (Table 3).

Table 3 Association between quartiles of FPG and cognitive function

Combined association of FPG and SUA with cognitive function

The combined association of SUA and FPG with cognitive function was significant in global cognition and episodic memory after adjusting for additional confounders (Table 4). After stratification by sex, the combination of lower SUA and higher FPG in females was associated with poorer performance in global cognition (β = -0.983, 95% CI: -1.563–-0.402) and episodic memory (β = -0.666, 95% CI: -1.072–-0.259). The two models with different adjustments were significant (Table 5).

Table 4 Association between combination of FPG and SUA quartiles and cognitive function
Table 5 Association between combination of FPG and SUA quartiles and cognitive function (stratified by sex)

Sensitivity analysis

According to the American diabetes association, diabetes is diagnosed if any of the following criteria are met: (1) FPG > 7.0 mmol/L; (2) random blood glucose > 11.1 mmol/L; (3) HBA1c > 6.5%; (4) self-reported diabetes diagnosed by physicians; and (5) intake of antidiabetic drugs or insulin therapy [18]. Although the association between DM and cognitive performance was not statistically significant in either men or women (Tables S2 and Table S3), in the female group, the global cognition performance and episodic memory performance of patients with DM and low SUA was poorer than those without DM but low SUA (Table S4), similar to the main results. In addition, the results of the association of SUA and FPG as continuous variables with cognitive scores are presented in the supplementary material (Tables S5 and Table S6).

Discussion

This study examined the relationship of SUA, FPG levels, and DM to cognitive function among middle-aged and older Chinese population. A low SUA level was associated with poor cognition, but no significant relationship was observed between FPG or DM and cognitive function. Higher FPG level combined with lower SUA level was related to poorer cognitive performance among female participants. Similar results were found in patients with DM and low SUA. The association remained significant after adjusting for a wide array of health-related variables.

The findings of our research are consistent with those of previous studies, which demonstrated the association between low SUA levels and poor cognitive function. For example, a prospective population cohort study of 4,618 participants aged 55 years and above found that higher SUA levels were associated with a lower risk of dementia; as such, a high SUA level was a protective factor for cognitive function [19]. A meta-analysis reported that SUA levels were higher in healthy controls without dementia but lower in patients with dementia [20]. This result is consistent with our findings, that is, a low SUA level was associated with poor cognition. The insignificant association between FPG and cognitive function is also in line with previous results. A Finnish National 2000 Health Examination Survey and a subsequent 11-year follow-up study (baseline included 3695 participants, mean age 49.3, range 30–86 years, 55.5% women, participants who were treated with insulin or unknown diabetes medication were excluded) also found that FPG levels were not associated with cognitive function [21]. However high blood sugar levels can cause cognitive impairment in individuals with diabetes; cognitive decline occurs in areas such as memory, orientation, and executive function [22]. The relationship between plasma glucose, especially outside of diabetes and within the reference range, to cognitive functions remains less clear. A previous study of elderly people (≥ 55 years of age) in a Chinese community found that normal FPG, impaired FPG, and glucose in a diabetic state were not associated with the degree of cognitive dysfunction (as graded by MMSE scores) [23]. Moreover, blood glucose was not associated with cognitive performance in participants without cognitive impairment [12].These inconsistencies in the association between plasma glucose and cognition could be attributed to the interaction between FPG and SUA.

As the metabolic end product of purines [24], the homeostasis of SUA is jointly balanced by endogenous production, exogenous supply, excretion and reabsorption. Xanthine oxidoreductase is the enzyme directly responsible for the conversion of purine bases to SUA. The endogenous production of SUA mainly occurs in tissues with high expression of xanthine oxidoreductase. In humans, the epithelial cells of liver, gastrointestinal tract, kidney and lactation mammary gland have the expression of this enzyme [25], and the organs with the highest expression activity are the liver and small intestine [26]. The exogenous supply of SUA is mainly derived from dietary behaviors such as high-purine foods and alcohol intake, which cause an increase in SUA of about 1-2 mg/dL [27]. SUA excretion and reabsorption efficiencies depend on the associated transport system. SUA is mainly excreted by the kidneys (about 70%), and the rest (about 30%) is excreted by the feces of the intestine. Twice reabsorptions of uric acid by the kidneys, resulting in 5 to 15% of the initial uric acid being excreted in the urine [28]. The difference in the normal physiological range of SUA between the sexes may be due to potential sex differences in these mechanisms. One study found that plasma xanthine oxidoreductase activity was higher in females than in males [29]. And the increase in SUA caused by a high-purine diet is more likely to occur in men [30]. The higher prevalence of gastrointestinal disease in Chinese elderly was found in women in this study (Table S1). In addition, there were gender differences in renal function impairment [31]. These potential sex differences in the mechanisms by which SUA maintains homeostasis provide an important basis for our sex-stratified analysis.

The potential risk effect of low SUA on cognitive domains may be explained by several mechanisms. Many studies have demonstrated the antioxidant properties of SUA over the decades. Urate accounts for about half of the antioxidant capacity of human plasma, and its antioxidant properties are as powerful as ascorbic acid [27, 28]. The main function of SUA is to remove reactive oxygen species and peroxynitrite; urate also protects the human blood from iron-mediated oxidation of ascorbate [32, 33]. In addition, under a phylogenetic perspective, the inability to catabolize uric acid might have conferred an advantage against age-related diseases mediated by high circulating SUA levels [34]. The intricate mechanisms of SUA in oxidative stress are important. The SUA level may affect the oxidative activity in the brain to a certain extent, and oxidative stress is a significant cause of cognitive dysfunction [35]. SUA is a potent antioxidant that has been studied for potential neuroprotective treatment [36]. The mechanism of action of SUA appears pleiotropic and is not only confined to a redox paradigm. Another study found that lower levels of SUA were associated with lower brain metabolism related to cognitive disorder [37]. These findings are the basic hypothesis of the research on SUA and cognitive function.

The combination of low SUA and high FPG or DM was associated with poor cognitive function among the female participants. A low level of SUA is a potential risk factor for the nervous system due to its antioxidant properties. Hyperglycemia increases the body's additional redox stress burden [38]. In animal models of DM, hyperglycemia induces a redox imbalance (an increase in the NADH/NAD+ ratio due to the oxidation of NADH to NAD+), which in turn adversely affects vascular and neurological function [39]. This redox stress depletes the body of other antioxidants [40]. Therefore, lack of SUA is more dangerous during hyperglycemia, and the co-existence of the two potential cognitive risk factors may result in poor cognitive outcome. This finding suggests that a moderate increase in SUA is associated with better cognitive function in female patients with higher blood glucose levels. The high glucose state increases the number of oxygen free radicals, induces endothelial cell apoptosis, restricts cerebrovascular production, and affects cerebral blood supply [41], leading to neurological dysfunction [42, 43]. In hyperglycemia, endothelial nitric oxide synthase is susceptible to uncoupling. When uncoupling occurs, NAD(P)H reacts with O2 and endothelial cells produce superoxide (O2•) instead of protective endothelial NO [44]. NO protects the antioxidant properties of SUA [45],so decreased NO production during hyperglycemia may lead to a reduced antioxidant contribution of SUA. This phenomenon is a possible explanation for the lack of protective cognitive effect of high FPG combined with high SUA.

The association of high FPG and low SUA with poor cognition was observed in women only, indicating that elderly women with high FPG are more in need of high SUA for antioxidant. Corroborating this point of view, a study in premenopausal women after hysterectomy, oviduct, and ovariectomy reported that the gene expression of superoxide dismutase and glutathione peroxidase is estrogen dependent [46]. Animal experiments in rats showed increased activity of NADPH oxidase (which promotes reactive oxygen species generation) when estrogen is absent [47]. Decreases in estrogen level can change the expression of important oxidative stress-related enzymes, causing heavy oxidative stress among elderly women. The APOE4 allele is the most important genetic factor that increases the risk of Alzheimer's disease [48]. An animal model study found that female APOE4 carriers had higher levels of oxidative stress in their brains, especially at synaptic terminals [49]. A human RNAseq analysis also showed that oxidative stress-related genes were highly expressed in female APOE4 carriers [50]. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) showed that female APOE4 carriers had weaker brain connectivity in the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex compared with male APOE4 carriers [51]. Studies of APOE4 support indicated that the female brain had a higher risk of oxidative stress than the male brain. Female diabetic rats had significantly higher levels of NADPH oxidase 1 and NADPH oxidase 4 than female non-diabetic rats and male diabetic or non-diabetic rats [52]. The results suggest that high glucose level and female gender are important risk factors for oxidative stress. In addition, white matter lesions are a pathological substrate for cognitive impairment [53], as individuals with cognitive impairment were found to have more periventricular white matter hyperintensities (WMH) on the MRI scans [54]. People with higher FPG had more WMH than those with normal FPG [55]. The difference in the results between sexes is because female have more WMH [56]. The sex difference is also particularly pronounced for periventricular WMH [57]. Thus, high blood sugar levels may be an important risk factor for cognitive function in women. In conclusion, the antioxidant effects of high SUA may be particularly important for elderly female participants with high FPG level and oxidative stress and low antioxidant defenses. Men have a higher risk of cerebrovascular diseases than women [58]; as such, the higher SUA-related cerebrovascular burden may counteract the antioxidant effects of high SUA, which may explain why the results were not significant in men.

This study has some advantages. First, the results provided additional evidence to explore the potential relationship of FPG and SUA to cognitive decline. Second, CHARLS contained a number of possible confounders potentially affecting cognitive function, and these covariables were reasonably adjusted through multi-step analysis. Third, in addition to quaternized SUA and FPG, sensitivity analyses were conducted for the combination of DM and low SUA, thereby improving the robustness of the results. However, some limitations should be considered. First, this work was an observational study. Thus, no clear cause-and-effect relationship could be obtained. Second, the included individuals had significantly higher educational levels and cognitive scores than the excluded CHARLS participants (Table S1). Thus, our findings are suggestive only for participants with better cognitive and educational levels. Third, measurement of SUA and FPG levels only once may not be sufficient to accurately estimate a representative concentration level for a person over time. Fourth, although cognitive function in several cognitive domains was measured, it may still be relatively limited and the effect of SUA/blood glucose levels on other cognitive domains, such as executive function, remains unclear. Finally, we lacked information on the structure of the high-purine diet of the participants.

Lower SUA was associated with poorer cognitive function in women with higher blood sugar levels. The small β in our study is different from previous studies, suggesting that a change in SUA of one quartile is associated with a small change in cognitive function, which may not be clinically applicable but can be predictive. Further research is warranted to examine whether or not combined interventions for controlling SUA in a slightly higher range and FPG in a slightly lower range could translate into clinical benefits for the protection of cognition.