Introduction

Environmental lead exposure is associated with a wide range of neurodevelopmental deficits in children, with no threshold below which lead does not have adverse effects [1,2,3,4,5,6,7] Chronic malnutrition is also associated with cognitive deficits [8,9,10,11]. Children who experience food insecurity and are subjected to environmental contaminants, such as metals, may be particularly prone to neurodevelopmental delays and other negative health outcomes [12].

One common indicator of a nutritional deficit is stunted growth, which occurs when a child’s height (or length) for a given age is below the 5th percentile [13]. Current estimates show that the world’s poorest children have stunting rates more than double those of the richest [14, 15]. Similar trends are seen in the global burden of lead exposure, with children in developing countries at the greatest risk of exposure to this heavy metal [16]. Such findings highlight the strong likelihood of concurrent exposures, yet to our knowledge, only one study has investigated the joint effects and interactions of co-exposure to lead and stunting on neurodevelopment; no significant stunting-by-metal interactions for lead, arsenic, and manganese were reported [12].

Children in Bangladesh experience both stunting and exposure to metals [12, 17]. Thirty-six percent of children under the age of 5 years in Bangladesh are stunted [11], and stunting rates within the country are higher as socioeconomic status declines [17]. At the district administrative level, stunting rates range from 28 to 51%, and over half of the districts in Bangladesh (39/64) have stunting rates greater than the World Health Organization’s (WHO) critical threshold of 40% [18]. Blood metal levels, such as blood lead, are not surveyed nationally; however, previous research found that 78% of children from urban and rural areas had blood lead levels above the CDC reference level of 5 μg/dL [19] and 87% of children at a primary school in Dhaka had BLLs ≥ 10 μg/dL [20]. In contrast, approximately 97.5% of children in the USA have blood lead levels below the current reference level of 5 μg/dL [21].

Stunting may be a mediator of the lead exposure-cognitive development relationship, or part of the causal pathway. Many studies have shown an inverse correlation between blood lead concentration and children’s height [19, 22, 23], with postulation that lead causes decreases of gonadotropin secretion, and that abnormalities in growth hormone axis can contribute to adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes [24]. Stunting might also act as an effect modifier that is the adverse effect of lead is greater among children with stunting. One potential mechanism for this is increased absorption of lead: children with compromised nutritional status may absorb more metals into their bodies compared to children with adequate nutritional intake, leading to greater declines in neurodevelopment [25, 26]. Additionally, both lead and malnutrition affect neuronal metabolism. The combination could lead to greater declines in neurodevelopment. Understanding whether growth is an effect modifier or mediator is important to design effective interventions to improve neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Mediation analysis is an established method used in social and epidemiological studies to understand the impact of variables in causal pathways or biological mechanisms. New statistical methods have been developed to understand the contributions of effect modification and mediation in epidemiological studies using a causal framework [27,28,29]. Using mediation analysis, the effect of lead can be decomposed into the component due to only mediation through stunting (the pure indirect effect), only interaction between lead and stunting (the reference interaction), both (the mediated interaction), or neither (the controlled direct effect) [30]. Past research in this cohort has found negative effects of metals on neurodevelopment, both in individual concentrations and in mixtures [30].

The aim of this study was to evaluate the role of stunting on the relationship between lead exposure and neurodevelopment scores using a Bangladeshi birth cohort. Our previous studies from this cohort revealed an association between prenatal lead exposure and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in early childhood [31], a finding that is consistent with decades of research. We have also used data from this cohort to understand the effect of metal mixtures on neurodevelopment [30, 32], to apply new statistical methods for the study of multi-pollutant mixtures [33], and to validate gene-environment studies [34]. In this analysis, we hypothesize that stunting acts as a mediator of environmental lead exposure. If our hypothesis is correct, one implication is that interventions targeted to improve chronic malnutrition would be effective in reducing the adverse effects of environmental lead exposure. Our previous studies have shown lead is associated with decreased cognitive scores in children and its effect is greater than those of other environmental metals. We have also shown that when blood lead levels are high, lead is associated with decreased cognitive scores and effects of other metals are not detected [31]. Therefore, in this study, we focus on lead rather than on environmental mixtures.

Methods

The study population has been described previously [30, 31, 35, 36]. Briefly, between 2008 and 2011, pregnant women in the Sirajdikhan (alternate spelling Serajdikhan) and Pabna upazilas (administrative regions) of Bangladesh served by Dhaka Community Hospital (DCH) were enrolled and followed through pregnancy and delivery. Children were followed at birth, age 12 months, and age 20–40 months.

The research team obtained informed consent from the parents of all children. The project was approved by the Human Research Committees at the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health (HSPH) and DCH. The Committee for Clinical Investigation at Boston Children’s Hospital (BCH) ceded review of this study to HSPH.

We enrolled 1613 pregnant women in the birth cohort study. Of these, 1153 pregnancies resulted in singleton live births and 5 pregnancies resulted in twin births. Of the remaining women, 123 were lost to follow-up, 125 withdrew from study activities, 132 pregnancies resulted in miscarriage, and 75 pregnancies resulted in stillbirth. A total of 1153 women and their children participated in follow-up studies, including 815 children who participated in neurodevelopmental assessments at age 20–40 months. Complete data were available for 734 children, and this is the sample used for analysis.

Environmental exposure assessment

Exposure to lead, arsenic, and manganese was assessed using blood samples collected at two time points: at birth using the umbilical cord blood and at 20–40 months of age collected via venipuncture, representing early childhood exposure. We collected the blood in trace metal-free tubes. The umbilical cord blood samples were analyzed at the Trace Metals Laboratory at HSPH using ICP-MS. Venous blood samples were analyzed at HSPH and the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai using similar protocols [30]. The average limit of detection (LOD) for lead and arsenic was 0.01 μg/dL; the LOD for manganese was 0.05 μg/dL.

Stunting

Trained field staff measured the weight, length, and head circumference of the newborns and children [32]. We measured height and head circumference to the nearest 0.1 cm and weight to the nearest 0.1 kg. We calculated z-scores for child’s height-for-age using the WHO’s Multicentre Growth Reference Study (MGRS) [13]. These standards provide a single international standard of physiological growth for all children from birth to 5 years of age. Specific computations were performed with the WHO Anthro and Macros (Version 3.2.2). Children with height-for-age less than or equal to 2 z-scores below the median of the WHO’s child growth standards were classified as stunted.

Neurodevelopmental outcomes

We translated the Bayley Scales of infant and Toddler Development™, Third Edition (BSID-III™) and adapted it for use in rural Bangladesh [37]. The BSID-III was administered by trained study personnel blinded to participants’ environmental exposure measures. The quality control included review of all BSID-III videotapes by site leaders (Ibne Hasan and Halder), frequent observed assessments (Mazumdar), and review of 5% of the videotaped administrations of the BSID-III by a senior neuropsychologist (Bellinger).

Covariates

We collected demographic information using structured questionnaires to obtain data on age, education, tobacco exposure, socioeconomic status, and dietary information [19]. Maternal height and weight were recorded at the initial study enrollment visit during the 1st trimester of pregnancy. Maternal education was recoded and collapsed into a dichotomous variable (primary education or less/more than a primary education) due to small numbers in the higher education categories. This categorization is consistent with other studies from Bangladesh [38,39,40]. We used a locally validated dish-based semi-quantitative food questionnaire (FFQ) to collect dietary information [41]. A measure of maternal protein intake during pregnancy was derived from the FFQ and coded to indicate weekly protein consumption of fish, meat, and eggs as high (> 26 units), medium (12.5–26 units), or low (< 12.5 units). Interviewers also administered the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Inventory [42]. This inventory was previously translated and adapted for use in Bangladesh [43]. Mothers’ IQ was estimated using Raven’s Progressive Indices [44].

Statistical methods

We performed statistical analyses using RStudio version 1.2.5033 (using R version 4.0.1) [45] and Stata version 16 [46]. We examined distributional plots and descriptive statistics for all variables and performed additional analyses stratified by clinic site. We evaluated for potential selection bias by comparing baseline characteristics of the birth cohort to those of the study sample. We modeled blood metal concentrations as natural log-transformed continuous variables. This transformation achieves a common scale and accounts for the right-skewedness of the biomarker levels. One study ID had a blood sample below the LOD for venipuncture lead, arsenic, and manganese at the 20–40 months of visit. We used one half of the LOD for this observation. Cognitive scores were approximately normally distributed and were modeled as continuous outcomes.

Mediation analysis

We conducted mediation analysis to evaluate whether stunting was acting as a mediator of the relationship between cord blood lead concentration and cognitive test scores in early childhood using the 4-way decomposition proposed by VanderWeele (Fig. 1). The mediation analysis was conducted using the med4way command in Stata [28, 32], a package that our group developed previously for mediation analysis. For this specific study, we tested the effect of a change in cord blood lead from the 25th to the 75th percentile. As stunting was a dichotomous variable, the parameter m, denoting the level of the mediator, was set to 0 to estimate the controlled direct effect (the effect of lead that is neither due to mediation nor interaction). The analyses require adjustment for exposure-outcome, exposure-mediator, mediator-outcome confounders. All other covariates were held at their respective means. Using this mediation analysis, the effect of lead can be decomposed into the component due to only mediation through stunting (the pure indirect effect), only interaction between lead and stunting (the reference interaction), both (the mediated interaction), or neither (the controlled direct effect) [30]. Mean, median, and confidence interval estimates for each component of the decomposition were obtained for each site.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Diagram showing the mediation model components testing the effect of cord blood lead on cognitive score with stunting status as a mediator, following a previous directed acyclic graph [28]

The covariates used in the model were selected to meet the criteria for using the mediation analysis [28], including all confounders of exposure-mediator, exposure-outcome, and mediator-outcome are adjusted for in the model. Additionally, there must be no mediator-outcome confounders affected by the exposure. Unadjusted models were analyzed for each of these components and possible confounders were added to a subsequent adjusted model. New adjusted models were created for each variable of interest. We assumed a variable was a confounder is the effect estimate for the predictor changed by greater than 10%.

Regression models and interaction analyses

Mediation analysis requires the specification of a model for the outcome (cognitive score) and a model for the mediator (stunting). A multivariable linear regression model was used to model the mean of cognitive score conditionally on cord blood lead, stunting, and confounders. A logistic regression model was used to model the conditional odds of stunting score given cord blood lead and confounders. The following covariates were included in models to evaluate for confounding the study site, child’s sex, child’s birth length, child birth weight, child’s age at the time of neurodevelopmental testing, mother’s age at the time of the child’s birth, maternal education, maternal IQ, maternal height and weight, HOME score, secondhand smoke exposure, maternal protein intake, cord blood arsenic and manganese, and early childhood blood metal concentrations. To identify if squared terms were needed in the regression model, we used the generalized additive model (gam) spline function in R’s mgcv package (Mixed GAM computation vehicle) [47]. The criteria for remaining in the model consisted of a p value less than 0.05 and an effective degrees of freedom (EDF) greater than 1. Given the important differences in exposure profiles and baseline characteristics observed across the two study sites, regression models were stratified by clinic in primary analyses and adjusted for in secondary analyses. We evaluated for effect modification by stunting status using interaction terms in the regression models. We performed additional analyses using the z-score length-for-age as a continuous variable for child growth.

Results

Study population

A summary of the study population is presented in Table 1. In general, the population was primarily agricultural/rural with low rates of formal education for mothers. There were important differences between the sites. Children from Sirajdikhan had higher blood lead concentrations in cord blood samples (median 6.1 vs. 1.7 μg/dL) and at 20–40 months (median 8.2 vs. 4.0 μg/dL) than children from Pabna. Children from Sirajdikhan also had higher rates of stunting than children from Pabna (66% vs. 40%), although the mothers in Sirjadikhan were generally heavier (median 47 vs. 45 kg). Mothers in Pabna were more likely to consume high amounts of protein during pregnancy compared to mothers in Sirajdikhan (44% vs 3%). We did not find significant differences in baseline characteristics between the full original cohort and the complete case study sample used in this analysis.

Table 1 Study population

Mediation analysis

We performed stratified analyses by site due to concerns about residual confounding, a strategy that is consistent with other reports from this cohort [31, 48]. A table reporting the 4 decomposed effects is shown in Table 2. In Pabna, where lead exposure was generally lower, the mediation analysis demonstrated a significant effect resulting from the reference interaction (the effect due only to interaction) between cord blood lead concentration and stunting, which reduced cognitive scores − 0.779 (p = 0.001). We did not find evidence of a reference interaction in Sirajdikhan (p = 0.483). No significant effect was found for mediation only, mediation and interaction combined, or neither mediation nor interaction in either site. Collectively, these results suggest the presence of effect modification by stunting in an area with low-level lead exposure.

Table 2 Mediation analysis of the effect of log cord blood lead concentrations on cognitive scores when mediated by stunting status at both study sites

Effect modification

In our regression models, we confirmed a significant interaction between lead and stunting status, both in the cohort as a whole and in our stratified analyses. In the 3-way model for the entire cohort, study site had a main effect on cognitive scores (β = − 2.77, SE = 1.15, p = 0.016) with Sirajdikhan coded as study site 0 and Pabna coded as study site 1. The results for the 3-way interaction confirmed a significant interaction for log cord blood lead (μg/dL)*stunting*study site (β = − 1.67, SE = 0.89, p = 0.06) on cognitive scores. In our stratified models, in Pabna, an area where lead levels were low, we found a 1-unit increase in natural log cord blood lead μg/dL in the presence of stunting was associated with a 2.10 decrease in cognitive scores (p = 0.003, standard error = 0.713). In Sirajdikhan, where blood lead concentrations were generally higher, this interaction was not found (Table 3). In sensitivity analysis, we found a significant interaction of log cord blood lead (μg/dL)*z-score height-for-age in Pabna (β = 0.86, SE = 0.39, p = 0.03) and results that approached statistical significance in Sirajdikhan (β = 0.42, SE = 0.22, p = 0.06) (Table 3). These findings confirm our hypothesis that child growth is an important effect modifier of lead.

Table 3 Regression results for the effect of cord blood lead concentration and childhood stunting on cognitive neurodevelopment at study sites

Discussion

In this study, we applied causal mediation analysis to assess the contribution of stunting, a measure of impaired growth related to poor nutrition, to the adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes associated with environmental lead exposure. We found that stunting modifies the adverse effects of lead, such that the adverse effect of lead was greater among children who were stunted. Effect modification was most apparent among children with low-level lead exposure. The results suggest that a child experiencing stunting with a log-cord blood lead concentration of 4 μg/dL may have cognitive scores 8.4 points lower than a child with similar cord blood lead exposure but without stunting. Our analyses did not reveal evidence of mediation, suggesting that stunting itself it does not lie within the causal pathway between lead exposure and adverse cognitive development.

Dietary interventions have long been part of a comprehensive strategy to prevent and mitigate the neurodevelopmental effects of environmental lead exposure in children [49]. These strategies are based on observations that blood lead is associated with lower intake of iron [50], calcium [51], vitamin C [52], and D[53] and that blood lead concentration among children is associated decreased height of children [22]. These observational studies are limited in determining causality and in answering important questions needed to design effective interventions [54, 55].

In this study, we apply a novel statistical method that simultaneously evaluates for mediation and effect modification [27, 28]. In our example, while we observe that stunted children are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of low-level, we also determine that stunting is unlikely to be part of the mechanism by which lead causes adverse cognitive development. These results suggest that resources might best be used to screen for lead exposure among populations with high rates of stunting and reduce lead exposure in these populations.

Strengths and limitations

Our studies used biomarkers of metal exposure using ICP-MS, a gold standard for the measure of blood lead concentration and a measure not widely available in low-resource settings such as Bangladesh. In addition to lead, we were able to control for exposure to other toxic metals in our models, which is an additional strength. Understanding the effects of metal mixtures was not the focus of this analysis. Additionally, we were able to adjust for a large number of potential confounders as well as predictors of stunting and neurodevelopment. However, we recognized the potential for unmeasured confounding variables and stratified analyses by clinic reduced the heterogeneity in demographics and potential for residual confounding.

Conclusion

Causal mediation analysis enables investigation of potential mechanisms of disease in epidemiological studies. We used a novel method of mediation analysis to test whether stunting, a measure height-for-age related to poor nutrition, mediated the adverse effect of prenatal lead exposure on cognitive outcomes in Bangladesh. While we did not find that stunting acted as mediator, we found significant effect modification by stunting. Our study suggests that stunted children are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of low-level lead exposure, interventions that include targeting populations with high rates of stunting for lead screening and reduction of lead exposure are most likely to be effective in improving neurodevelopmental outcomes.