Background

Behavior involves the perception and processing of sensory information into a signaling cascade that mediates physiological and motor outputs. This complex process is influenced by an organism's environment, genetic make-up and nervous system function. Social interactions influence an organism's behavior [15], and these behavioral changes are associated with alterations in morphology [69] and gene expression [6, 1017]. However, the mechanisms mediating the changes are unclear. As we work to understand responses to behavior at the transcript level, we can clarify the regulatory and intracellular processes governing nervous system function and behavior.

Therefore, we are studying reproductive behaviors in the genetically tractable Drosophila melanogaster, which exhibit stereotypical mating behaviors [reviewed in [18, 19]] regulated by genetics [reviewed in [20, 21]] and social interactions [[1, 22, 23]; reviewed in [19, 24, 25]]. The sex-determination gene hierarchy is the major regulator of Drosophila reproduction [reviewed in [26, 27]]. Components of this pathway affect sexually dimorphic development, including the neural circuitries necessary for sex-specific courtship behaviors [2832]. However, the behavioral functions of only a few of the downstream target genes of the hierarchy are known [3343].

Although the potential for performing courtship behavior is under genetic control, experience with other individuals alters behavior, particularly in the context of courtship learning [19, 24, 25]. During courtship and mating, the male is inundated with sensory information that must be interpreted so that the appropriate signals are sent throughout the body for a successful mating. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that a more experienced male would be better at performing some aspect of courtship to improve his mating success. In support of this idea, Drosophila males experienced at courting females initiate courtship toward novel, receptive females more quickly than do inexperienced males [44, 45]. In a natural setting where many flies are competing for mates, rapid courtship initiation may give an experienced male a competitive advantage that increases his mating success. Simply observing courtship and mating behavior of other flies is not sufficient to decrease the male's own mating latency, indicating that this learning behavior requires active participation [45]. It is possible that changes in courting and mated male gene expression underlie this decreased courtship latency in subsequent interactions.

By combining behavioral assays with microarray technology, it is possible to assess behaviorally-responsive gene expression changes on a genome-wide scale [12, 22, 4651] to find loci regulating or regulated by behavior, including sex-determination hierarchy target genes. Prior work in our lab demonstrated that males rapidly alter gene expression at the whole-animal level during courtship [12, 22]. Next, we focused on changes occurring in the male head as a result of mating since these changes likely affect function of the nervous system and other reproductively important tissues to promote reproductive success. Our study demonstrates that courtship culminating in mating affects gene expression patterns in male heads and that many of the gene products are expressed in non-neural adipose tissue that may play an important modulatory role in neural function and behavior.

Results and Discussion

Mating causes expression changes in male heads

Gene expression levels change rapidly as males court females [12, 22]. To determine the effects of courtship culminating in mating on male gene expression, we compared transcriptional profiles of males that mated with a female to those that were not presented with a female (control). Labeled samples from control and treatment groups were hybridized to Drosophila Genome 2.0 Arrays (Affymetrix, Santa Clara, CA, USA), which are based on the Flybase 3.1 annotation, targeting nearly 18,500 transcripts.

In the current study we focused on head expression, rather than whole body expression [12, 22], to identify gene expression changes in the nervous system and other tissues within the head (such as sensory systems and fat body) that likely modulate reproduction. We isolated male heads (rather than dissecting out the brains) since accumulating evidence from our lab [[12, 22]; L.L. Ellis and G.E. Carney, unpublished results] as well as from other published studies [[37, 39, 40, 43]; reviewed in [52]] indicates that head tissues, such as the fat body surrounding the brain (Fig. 1), likely also have important modulatory functions in behavior. To have the potential to identify gene expression changes in these tissues as well, we elected to assay the entire male head for alterations in gene expression patterns in response to mating.

Figure 1
figure 1

Fat body tissue in the adult male. Low magnification image (10×) from the front of an adult male head (A) or dorsal abdominal cuticle (E). Boxed areas indicate adipose tissue magnified at 20× in 3 areas of the head (B-D) and 2 areas along the abdominal cuticle (F).

We used five algorithms to extract expression values from each array and performed paired t-test comparisons between the expression values derived from mated male head arrays and control male head arrays. Using this strategy we identified 47 mating-responsive genes (See Methods). Two hours after mating with a female, males significantly up regulated 25 genes (Table 1) and down regulated 22 genes (Table 2). Such changes are not likely to be activity-dependent since control males had locomotor levels similar to males that courted females (two-tailed t-test, p > 0.05).

Table 1 Candidate genes up regulated 2 hrs after mating
Table 2 Candidate genes down regulated 2 hrs after mating

Verification of microarray results by independent qPCR

To confirm the microarray results, we performed qPCR analysis on independently collected mated and control male head RNA samples. We tested a subset of genes whose expression levels changed significantly in mated male heads compared to control male heads. Eight out of 10 up-regulated genes and 2 out of 3 down-regulated genes had the expected directional change (Table 3). We did not verify up regulation of CG4825 and fit or down regulation of CG8112 by qPCR. However, increased fit expression in the fat body lining the brain was confirmed by in situ hybridization (see below).

Table 3 Confirmation of microarray results by qPCR

Expression of candidate genes is not restricted to the brain

We hypothesized that examining gene expression in head tissue instead of whole bodies would uncover genes that function in reproduction by regulating nervous system signaling. This could be via direct effects on neural gene expression or by effects on other tissues in the head that receive or respond to courtship and mating signals. We found that expression of many mating-responsive genes is enriched in the head but not the brain (Table 4) [53], indicating expression occurs outside of the brain. While some of the genes are expressed in the eye, others appear enriched in tissues other than the brain and eye.

Table 4 Candidate genes are enriched in head tissue other than the brain, including adult adipose tissue

One possibility is that they are expressed in an adipose tissue called the fat body that lines the head cavity surrounding the brain (Fig. 1a-d) and is implicated in courtship behavior modulation [[37, 39, 40, 43]; reviewed in [52]]. Data showing that mating-responsive genes enriched in the head are also enriched in the adult fat body (Table 4) [53] support this hypothesis. In situ hybridization confirmed that several mating-responsive loci (CG13360, bubblegum (bgm), Prx2540-2, CG8449 and CG4825) are expressed in male fat body tissue (Fig. 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Candidate genes are expressed in fat tissue. Antisense (A-D,I,K,M-Q) or sense (E-H,J,L,R-V) RNA probes were designed to cDNA clones for CG4825 (A,E,M,R), CG8449 (B,F,I,J,N,S), bgm (C,G,O,T), Prx2540-2 (D,H,K,L,P,U), and CG13360 (Q,V). In situ hybridization to whole-mount tissue showed candidate gene expression in male CS fat body tissue (arrows) present on head (A-H) and abdominal (M-V) cuticle. Purple reactivity indicates presence of transcripts. Brains (I-L) showed light pink background staining but lacked detectable expression of either CG8449 (I,J) or Prx2540-2 (K,L).

FlyAtlas data indicate that the fat-expressed genes CG13360, bgm, and Prx2540-2 are expressed at very low levels in brains, while CG8449 and CG4825 are expressed at low to moderate levels in the brain [53]. By in situ we did not detect brain expression of these five transcripts (Fig. 2 and data not shown), although we cannot rule out the possibility that low levels of message are present.

To test the hypothesis that mating-induced changes in gene expression occur in the fat body, we compared fit expression in the heads of mated and control heads. fit expression increased in the adipose tissue surrounding the male brain after courtship culminating in mating (Fig. 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

fit expression in the fat body is increased after courtship culminating in mating. In situ hybridization was performed on cryosectioned male heads and confirmed that fit transcript levels were up regulated in adipose tissue (arrows) of mated males (panel B) compared to control males (panel A) as indicted by increased intensity of purple staining in mated male fat body. Insets in panels A and B show magnified views of head fat. Qualitative assessment of signal intensity in both treatment groups indicates that fit expression increased in mated male heads (panel C).

Juvenile hormone esterases are important for male reproductive behaviors

We hypothesized that if a gene is up regulated after mating, that gene likely affects some aspect of reproductive behavior. Therefore, we assayed the percentage of time a male spent courting a female, known as the courtship index (CI), of candidate mating-responsive gene mutants. A Jhe P-element insertion, Jhee01859 , resulted in significantly reduced CI values in homozygous mutant males compared to heterozygous or wild-type controls (Fig. 4). Heterozygous males showed similar courtship activity compared to wild-type males, ruling out heterozygous effects on CI levels. Though Jhe males court females less vigorously, they perform standard courtship steps, eventually culminating in copulation.

Figure 4
figure 4

Jhe and clt mutants reduce courtship toward females. Mutant males homozygous for P-element insertions in Jhe or clt showed reduced courtship (***p < 0.001) under red light compared to sibling heterozygous and wild-type controls. Jhee01859 +/+ cltBG01317 mutant males showed significant reductions in courtship compared to either heterozygote or the wild-type control. (N) reflects sample size. Error bars are SEM.

In addition to Jhe there are three other candidate juvenile hormone esterase genes in the Drosophila genome [54]. One of the genes, cricklet (clt), also had an available P-element insertion, so we tested cltBG01317 mutants to see if they had a similar phenotype to Jhe mutants. We found that clt mutants also have decreased CIs relative to controls (Fig. 4). There is also a strong genetic interaction between Jhe and clt. Transheterozygous mutant males had significantly reduced courtship compared to controls (Fig. 4).

We predicted that mating-responsive loci would function to prime the male for subsequent mating encounters by regulating courtship or mating latency and duration. Therefore, we predicted that decreasing Jhe, which was up regulated in mated male heads, would increase courtship or mating latency. To test this hypothesis we examined the courtship and mating kinetics of mutants for Jhe and the related esterase clt. Courtship latency (time to initiation of courtship) did not differ among mutants and controls. Though Jhe and clt males mated with females, they had a significant (p < 0.05) increase in mating latency (Fig. 5) (ANOVA, genotype p < 0.05, trial p > 0.05), while mating duration was unaffected. The increased mating latency was not dependent on the mating trial (1st, 2nd or 3rd). However, as we increased the number of mating attempts, the mating success (as measured by the act of copulation) of Jhe and clt mutant males was significantly reduced (Fig. 6) (Binary Logistic Regression, genotype p < 0.01, trial p < 0.0001, interaction p < 0.0001) compared to heterozygous controls. Since we ruled out a heterozygous effect on CI values we did not test for heterozygous effects on mating latency or mating success. Females mated to Jhe or clt mutant males laid equivalent numbers of eggs regardless of the mating trial and day of egg laying (ANOVA, genotype p > 0.05, trial p > 0.05, day p > 0.05), and neither Jhe nor clt mutant females had detectable fertility defects.

Figure 5
figure 5

Mating latency is increased in Jhe and clt mutants. Under red light conditions, homozygous and transheterozygous mutant males had significantly (ANOVA p < 0.01, Tukey's *p < 0.05) increased mating latencies toward CS virgin females regardless of the mating bout (1st, 2nd, or 3rd); therefore, overall average mating latencies are shown. (N) reflects sample size. Error bars are SEM.

Figure 6
figure 6

Mating success decreases in Jhe and clt mutants. Jhe/+ and clt/+ control males successfully mated with 3 females in succession, while experimental Jhe and clt mutant males significantly (Binary Logistic Regression, genotype p < 0.01, trial p < 0.0001, interaction p < 0.0001) decreased their mating success with the 2nd and 3rd females. No. of successful matings/Total no. of pairings for 1st; 2nd; 3rd trials are shown.

Conclusions

Changes in gene expression upon mating

The complex reproductive behaviors exhibited by Drosophila require the interaction between genetics and environment. Courtship is an innate and stereotypical process under control of the somatic sex-determination hierarchy and is influenced by social interactions. Courtship and mating elicit gene expression changes in females [49, 5557], and courtship affects transcript profiles in males [12, 22]. The female post-mating effects occur rapidly (within minutes) or can be detected several hours after mating [49, 5557]. Within 5 min of courtship, whole-male gene expression profiles also change rapidly [12, 22]. In this study we expanded on our earlier studies in whole males to show that courtship culminating in mating causes changes in gene expression in the male head as well. Expression levels likely change rapidly in response to sensory cues received during courtship, while the physiological changes from mating [58] may mediate long-term expression level changes in the nervous system or elsewhere in the fly that can feed back to the nervous system.

The expression profile of a 5 min courting male differs from that of a 2 hr post-mating male. This is not surprising since we expected that the process of mating would have major effects on male physiology that would be reflected in altered transcriptional profiles. Of the 47 genes with altered expression 2 hrs after mating (Tables 1 and 2), only 1 gene, fit, is also up regulated in males after 5 min of courtship [12]. CG16772 is up regulated 2 hrs after mating but is down regulated during 5 min of courtship [12]. CG16772 is one of several fat body-expressed immune response genes down regulated during courtship, possibly to allow energetic resources to be directed toward offspring production rather than immunity [12, 22]. After mating, expression of CG16772 may increase because contact with a female increases the likelihood of encountering a pathogen.

The fact that few genes overlap between these data sets is not surprising since we assayed different time points (5 min or 2 hrs), different tissues (whole bodies in previous studies versus heads in this study) and different behaviors (courtship alone versus courtship culminating in mating). We also used different approaches for analyzing the data due to the differences in experimental design for each test. The analysis strategies provide us a conservative estimate of the transcripts affected by courtship and mating.

We predict that some mating-responsive genes facilitate an increased male mating efficiency for future encounters. Little is known about how repeated matings affect male mating latency, duration or fecundity. After his first mating, the male may perceive and process female stimuli more rapidly, may be more appealing to the female, or may be physiologically primed for subsequent matings by replenishment of Acps, sperm or other seminal proteins, resulting in decreased courtship or mating latencies. Alterations in gene expression, such as those described here and in our earlier work [12, 22], may contribute to these expected behavioral and physiological changes.

Gene expression in adipose tissue

The fat body is a secretory tissue [reviewed in [59]] whose effects on fly reproductive behavior have previously been described [[37, 39, 40, 43]; reviewed in [52]]. The majority of mating-responsive genes are expressed in adult adipose tissue (fat body) (Table 4), and we analyzed a subset of six up-regulated genes to show that they are expressed in adipose tissue surrounding the brain (Figs. 2 and 3). Furthermore, we observed increased expression of fit in male adipose tissue after courtship followed by mating (Fig. 3). fit also is expressed in the head fat of females and originally was named based upon its high expression in females under the control of Sex-lethal[39], which is the initial regulatory gene in the somatic sex-determination hierarchy.

Other studies also indicated that several mating-responsive genes identified in our study are expressed in the fat body surrounding the brain. Larval serum protein 2 (Lsp2) is expressed in the head fat of both sexes [60]. Of the 25 genes up regulated by courtship and mating, 14 are detectable (signal strength greater than 20) in brain and 21 genes are detectable in fat body based upon a microarray analysis of adult mRNA expression levels [53]. Of these 25 up regulated genes, 16 are enriched in the fat body relative to other adult tissues (Table 4).

Taken together, these results imply that the brain is not the only tissue responding to or regulating post-mating behavior, but that adipose tissue plays a role in this process as well. In response to mating, a signaling cascade initiated by neurosecretory cells may transmit the signal to the surrounding fat body. The fat body then could perpetuate the signal by secreting factors that influence neuronal or non-neuronal tissues. We hypothesize that expression level changes in the brain alter neuronal signaling either directly or indirectly, which impacts the processing of sensory cues and targets other reproductively important tissues.

Juvenile hormone esterases and male reproductive behavior

Another mating-responsive gene, Jhe, is also expressed in adipose tissue [6164] and functions in reproductive behavior (Figs. 4, 5, 6). Jhe and three closely related esterase genes (clt, Jhedup, and CG7529) have juvenile hormone esterase (JHE) activity in vitro. JHEs together with juvenile hormone epoxide hydrolases hydrolyze Juvenile hormone (JH) to regulate JH levels [65, 66]. Since Jhe expression is positively regulated by JH [67], the mating-induced increase in Jhe expression identified in our study may be JH dependent.

Much of our understanding of physiological functions of JH comes from studies investigating its function during development [reviewed in [68]]. However, JH also has important post-developmental functions such as promoting accessory gland protein (Acp) synthesis [69]. During mating Acps are transferred, along with sperm, to the female [70], and the transfer of Acps triggers male synthesis of new Acps [58]. Males also transfer Sex-peptide to the female during mating [7173]. Sex-peptide increases JH levels in females [74], which stimulates egg development [75]. However, possible mating-induced changes in male JH levels have not been evaluated. Since ejaculate components must be replenished after mating, we hypothesize that male JH levels increase after mating to stimulate Acp synthesis. The increase in JH would up regulate Jhe expression which would, in turn, reduce JH levels once the ejaculate components have been replenished.

JH also has a role in modulating behavior since males with reduced JH court females less intensely [76], Our data suggest that an increase in JH, caused by reduction of Jhe or clt, may also disrupt courtship (Figs. 4, 5, 6). Jhe and clt deficient males, which likely have increased levels of JH, court less vigorously (Fig. 4), have increased mating latencies (Fig. 5), and have reduced mating success (Fig. 6). This situation exemplifies the complex regulation governing behavior and implies that JH levels must be tightly regulated in order to ensure appropriate behavioral and physiological responses.

Gene expression in the brain

Although we are particularly interested in the large number of fat-expressed genes that were identified in this and earlier screens [12, 22], we also note that several of the identified transcripts are expressed in brains as would be expected for genes that function in behavior. Proper function of the nervous system relies on the appropriate cellular architecture, connections and signaling. Behavior requires the sensory systems to perceive the information accurately and transmit such information to the brain for processing. The brain can then transmit the signal to the appropriate output pathways which can modify signaling in tissues such as the fat body or the brain itself. Therefore the establishment and maintenance of the brain (and sensory systems) is vital to the organism's ability to respond to its environment and experiences. It is possible that mating-responsive genes act in the development or maintenance of a mated male brain as opposed to a naïve male brain.

Thirteen of the 21 fat-expressed genes up regulated in mated males are also expressed in brains at detectable levels [53]; a single transcript, CG4288 is detected in brains but not fat [53]. None of these genes have known function in behavior, but their reported mutant phenotypes or molecular functions indicate that several of the loci may have important neural maintenance functions.

For example, mutants for bgm, an enzyme involved in fatty acid metabolism that is expressed in both the brain and fat, have a neurodegeneration phenotype in response to accumulation of long chain fatty acids [77]. Another gene that potentially functions in a neurodegeneration pathway is Phosphatidyl-serine synthase, which responds to changes in polygluatmate (polyQ) levels [78]. polyQ diseases, including Huntington's Disease, are adult on-set progressive neural degeneration diseases caused by the accumulation of glutamate repeats [79].

Cellular homeostasis is important in the maintenance and function of the Drosophila brain. One gene that helps maintain this homeostasis is Iron regulatory protein 1B (Irp-1B) which encodes a protein that binds to iron-responsive elements (IREs) to regulate iron metabolism [80]. In addition to affecting cell survival and homeostasis, neural morphology might also be regulated by mating-responsive candidates. Mutants of Pabp2 show pathfinding and targeting defects in the larval neuromuscular junction [81].

Mating-responsive genes and the sex-determination hierarchy

This genome-wide analysis identified known sex-determination hierarchy target genes such as fit. Three other mating-responsive genes (CG16772, Prx2540-2, and CG16898) (Tables 1 and 2) are also regulated by the sex-determination hierarchy [41]. Transcriptional profiling of mutants for a variety of sex-determination hierarchy genes indicates that Prx2540-2 and CG16898 are regulated by fruitless (fru), while fit is downstream of transformer (tra). CG16772 may also function downstream of tra[41].

The splicing factor squid (sqd) is up regulated in mated male heads (Table 1). Interestingly, primary transcripts of the sqd locus are sex-specifically spliced in the head as well as the germline, although it is not known if sqd splicing is regulated by the sex-determination hierarchy [82]. It is possible that sqd and other mating-responsive loci function as downstream targets of the sex-determination hierarchy to regulate morphological and behavioral differences between male and female Drosophila. Alternatively, there may be other pathways (such as those that regulate alternative splicing) that function together with the sex-determination hierarchy to regulate reproductive behavior.

We predict that mating-responsive genes also function in other aspects of reproduction and behavior; therefore, we propose this genome-wide approach as a powerful tool for determining the genetic pathways and intracellular processes regulating reproduction, both at the behavioral and physiological levels.

Methods

Microarray Analysis

The wild-type Canton-S (CS) strain was isogenized to reduce genetic variation and the isoline was kept at 25°C on a 12-hr light/dark cycle. Twenty or fewer virgin CS males were aged collectively for 3 days at 25°C. On day 4, individual males were aspirated into vials. Virgin females were collected and aged in groups of 20 or fewer flies for 4 days at 25°C.

On day 5, males were equally divided into two treatment groups. One group (referred to as "mated males") consisted of individual males that were placed with a female for courtship and mating, while the second group of males (referred to as "control males") was mock exposed to a female. We tested both groups at the same time to allow for paired microarray and Cyber-T analyses (see below). For the mated male group, a single, aged virgin female was aspirated into each male's vial. The control males were treated identically except that no female was transmitted during the aspiration process.

Upon completion of mating, females were removed from the vials. Males from both treatment groups were quick frozen 2 hrs later and stored at -80°C for future RNA extraction; only pairs for which the mated male had a mating latency less than 30 min and mating duration of 18-30 min were collected for RNA extraction. Seventy-four percent of mated males tested met this requirement. All procedures were conducted at the same time each day to control for circadian effects.

Head tissue was separated from the remaining body by vortexing quick-frozen flies. Male heads were assigned to one of 20 groups (30 heads in each group; 10 mated and 10 control RNA preparations) so that control and mated samples collected together could be analyzed by paired statistical comparisons. Following standard protocols, total RNA from head tissue was extracted in Trizol (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). Total head RNA preparations from 10 groups (5 control groups and their corresponding mated groups) were sent to the University of Kentucky MicroArray Core Facility for labeling and hybridization to Affymetrix Drosophila 2.0 Genome Arrays following standard Affymetrix (Santa Clara, CA, USA) protocols.

Expression values were generated similarly to previous experiments [12, 22] using five algorithms (PM, PM-MM, MAS 5.0, GCRMA, and GeneSpring). Multiple expression value algorithms were used to control for variation among the algorithms and to generate a statistically stronger candidate gene set. We used dChip's PM (perfect match between the probe and target sequence) and PM-MM (one nucleotide between the probe and target sequence is mismatched) algorithms [83], as well as those implemented by GCOS (MAS 5.0, Affymetrix), R (GCRMA) [84], and GeneSpring (Agilent, Santa Clara, CA, USA). For the dChip algorithms, expression values were only considered if greater than 50; for the other 3 methods, expression values were required to be greater than 100. To test for significance, we used Cyber-T's Bayesian t-test analysis [85]. Candidate mating-responsive genes included those whose expression differed significantly (p < 0.001) between control male heads and mated male heads in at least 3 expression value data sets and had a false discovery rate less than 0.05 [86]. With such stringent criteria, we did not specify a particular fold change cut-off value.

qPCR

To confirm the microarray results, qPCR was performed on 10 independent samples (5 mated and 5 control RNA preparations) that were collected as described above but were not used in the microarray analysis. polyA+ RNA was isolated from each of the 10 samples using the Oligotex mRNA mini kit (Qiagen, Netherlands). cDNA was synthesized using the SuperScript First-Strand Synthesis System (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). We designed primers to amplify 10 up-regulated and 3 down-regulated genes, choosing genes that are predicted to be enriched in brain, fat body or both tissues based upon FlyAtlas expression data [53]. When possible, primer pairs were designed across introns to control for amplification specificity. Genes that are expressed at low levels in the head [[53]; L. L. Ellis and G. E. Carney, unpublished results] were not tested.

Using the SYBR Green PCR Mastermix (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA), 2 μL of a 1:4 dilution of each template was run in triplicate in the ABI7500 (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA) using default parameters. Control reactions lacking template and controls with template but without Reverse Transcriptase were used. Primer-specific amplification was determined by analyzing dissociation curves for each primer pair.

mRNA levels were determined by the Relative Standard Curve Method (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA), and candidate gene transcript levels were normalized to rp49 transcript levels. Normalizing the mated male transcript levels to the control male transcript levels generated a relative fold change. We also analyzed trends in the average relative transcript levels of each treatment (control and mated) using the two-tailed t-test. Secondary qPCR analysis confirmed increased expression of CG6188 and decreased expression of alpha Esterase-2.

Regression of mean expression microarray analysis fold changes compared to independent qPCR fold changes indicated a highly significant positive correlation between results obtained by the two methods (r = 0.51, N = 10, p = 0.021).

In situ hybridization

Digoxigenin (DIG)-labeled sense and antisense RNA probes were made from cDNA clones for six candidate genes with predicted fat body expression following the manufacturer's standard protocol (Roche, Nutley, NJ, USA). The genes and their corresponding cDNA clones were CG4825 (LD10327), CG8449 (GH10459), CG13360 (LP09811), bubblegum (bgm) (GM14009), fit (RH40291) and Prx2540-2 (RH69586). Expression of fit is regulated by tra while expression of Prx2540-2 is regulated by fruitless (fru) [41]; tra and fru are regulatory components of the sex-determination hierarchy. Antisense and sense probes were hydrolyzed into 200 bp fragments and in situ hybridization to male brains, head carcass and abdominal cuticle was performed as described in [33]. Antisense probes detected expressed transcripts in each case, while sense probes served as negative controls for expression.

To verify the increased expression of fit in male head tissue after courtship followed by mating, virgin CS males were collected 2 hrs after mating and compared to virgin CS control males that did not mate with a female. After treatment, males were cryosectioned in OCT compound and in situ hybridization was performed on the sections as described previously [37]. Control and mated tissues were placed on the same slide to control for histochemical reaction time. We qualitatively assessed fit expression in adipose tissue lining the brain from non-existent (-) to highly expressed (+++).

Courtship assays

All flies were kept on a 12-hr light/dark cycle at 25°C. P-element insertion mutations in Jhe and cricklet (clt) were obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (cltBG01317 ) and the Exelixis Collection at Harvard Medical School (Jhee01859 ). These insertions are likely hypomorphs since they are located in proximal promoter regions. Each P-element was backcrossed into the CS background to generate a genetically similar control that had one wild-type copy of Jhe or clt. To test for a genetic interaction between Jhe and clt, the two insertion strains, Jhee01859 and cltBG01317 , were crossed to generate transheterozygous flies containing a single P-element insertion in each gene (Jhee01859 +/+ cltBG01317 ). Virgin P-insertion or control males were collected and stored individually for 4 to 5 days; virgin CS females were aged collectively for 3 to 5 days.

Behavioral assays were conducted at 22°C under red light conditions to diminish the effect of eye color on vision and courtship. We video recorded the interactions with a digital camcorder so that subsequent analyses could be performed. To analyze courtship behavior, a male was aspirated into a mating chamber (diameter = 1 cm) and a virgin CS female was introduced 2 min later. The pair was video recorded for 10 min. The courtship index (CI; percentage of time the male spent performing courtship during the initial 10 min of observation) and courtship latency (time until courtship occurs) were calculated. CI values were arcsine transformed for statistical analysis. Two-tailed t-test comparisons between homozygous mutants and controls were calculated to determine significance (p < 0.05). Jhee01859 +/+ cltBG01317 males were compared to both control genotypes (two-tailed t-test).

Fertility Assays

The ability of a male to mate with multiple CS females and the fecundity of these matings was also assessed. Jhe and clt mutants and heterozygous controls, as well as CS virgin females, were collected and aged as described for the courtship assay. Under red light, a male was aspirated into a mating chamber, followed by a CS virgin female. The male was given 2 hrs to mate with the female. If mating occurred, the female was placed in a vial with food to measure fecundity (number of eggs laid and number of adult offspring) and the male was placed in a new mating chamber. A second CS virgin female was aspirated into the new chamber and the pair was given 2 hrs to mate. If the second mating occurred, the female was placed in a vial for later progeny counts, and the male was moved to another chamber for mating with a third and final female. The third mated female was also kept for further analysis. For the first mating bout, all 10 cltBG01317 males mated, while only three of the ten males mated with the second female and none of the 3 males mated with the third female. Eight out of 13 cltBG01317/+ males mated with the first female, five of those eight males mated with the second female and four of the remaining five males mated with the third female. Jhee01859 males only mated with the first female (six out of nine males). However, nine of ten Jhee01859/+ males mated with the first female, seven of those nine males mated with the second female and four of the seven males mated with the third female. For the transheterozygous cltBG01317/Jhee01859 males, seven of 12 mated with the first female, four of seven males mated with the second female and two of the four males mated with the third female.

The mating latencies and durations for each of the three possible matings were measured and significance was determined by Univariate ANOVA analysis using genotype and mating trial as fixed variables with Tukey's post-hoc analysis (SPSS). Males that did not mate within the 2 hr window were scored as being unsuccessful. Using linear regression, we assessed the significance (p < 0.05) of genotype and mating bout on mating success.

For 6 days following the assay, the female was transferred to a new vial and the number of eggs laid in each vial was determined. Vials were maintained at 25°C for 18 days to allow for a count of the total number of adult progeny. Significant effects of genotype and trial on mating latency or duration were measured by the Univariate ANOVA and Tukey's post-hoc analysis. We also measured the significance of genotype, mating bout and day of egg laying on the male's fecundity (Univariate ANOVA and Tukey's post-hoc analysis). Fecundity was measured by the total number of eggs laid and by the arcsine transformed ratio of adult offspring to eggs laid.