Skip to main content
Log in

Multivariate Intra-Sexual Selection on Men’s Perceptions of Male Facial Morphology

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Intra-sexual selection has shaped the evolution of sexually dimorphic traits in males of many primates, including humans. In men, sexual dimorphism in craniofacial shape (i.e. facial masculinity) and facial hair have both been shown to communicate aspects of social and physical dominance intra-sexually. However, less attention has been given to how variation in physical and social dominance among receivers impacts on perceptions of facial masculinity and beards as intra-sexual signals of formidability.

Methods

In the current study, male participants (N = 951) rated male faces varying in masculinity and beardedness when judging masculinity, dominance and aggressiveness. These participants also responded to scales measuring their psychological dominance, sexual jealousy, status seeking, and masculine morphology (facial masculinity, facial hair, and height).

Results

Beardedness exerted strong effects over clean-shaven faces on ratings of masculinity, dominance, and aggressiveness. Trait ratings of masculinity, dominance, and aggressiveness rose linearly with increasing craniofacial masculinity. The significant facial masculinity × facial hair interaction suggests that beardedness caused strong effects on all trait ratings over clean-shaven faces at every level of facial masculinity. Participants with full beards also reported higher scores on dominance and assertiveness scales. Participants high in dominance and assertiveness also gave higher ratings for dominance, but not masculinity or aggressiveness, to bearded over clean-shaven faces. Participants low in intra-sexual jealousy rated clean-shaven and/or feminised faces as less dominant, less masculine, and less aggressive.

Conclusions

These findings demonstrate that facial hair enhances perceptions of masculinity, dominance, and aggressiveness above ratings of facial masculinity, potentially by augmenting masculine craniofacial features. Individual differences in intra-sexual dominance showed associations with judgments of facial hair but not facial masculinity. Our study demonstrates that when two sexually dimorphic androgen dependent facial traits are judged in concert, ornamental rather than structural masculine facial features underpin men’s intra-sexual judgments of formidability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Addison, W. E. (1989). Beardedness as a factor in perceived masculinity. Perception and Motor Skills, 68, 921–922.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adhikari, K., Fontanil, T., Cal, S., Mendoza-Revilla, J., Fuentes-Guajardo, M., Chacón-Duque, J. C., et al. (2016). A genome-wide association scan in admixed Latin Americans identifies loci influencing facial and scalp hair features. Nature Communications, 7, 10815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 249–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnocky, S., Carré, J. M., Bird, B. M., Moreau, B. J., Vaillancourt, T., Ortiz, T., & Marley, N. (2018). The facial width-to-height ratio predicts sex drive, sociosexuality, and intended infidelity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(5), 1375–1385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2001). Mustache fashion covaries with a good marriage market for women. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 261–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J. (2013). Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson PJ, Perrett DI. 1993 Extracting prototypical facial images from exemplars. Perception, 22, 257–262.

  • Berglund, A., Bisazza, A., & Pilastro, A. (1996). Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 58, 385–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boothroyd, L. G., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., DeBruine, L. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Facial correlates of sociosexuality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(3), 211–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boothroyd, L. G., Cross, C. P., Gray, A. W., Coombes, C., & Gregson-Curtis, K. (2011). Perceiving the facial correlates of sociosexuality: Further evidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 422–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boothroyd, L. G., Scott, I., Gray, A. W., Coombes, C. I., & Pound, N. (2013). Male facial masculinity as a cue to health outcomes. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(5), 147470491301100508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borras-Guevara, M. L., Batres, C., & Perrett, D. I. (2017). Aggressor or protector? Experiences and perceptions of violence predict preferences for masculinity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(4), 481–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., Scott, I. M., Maklakov, A. A., Kasumovic, M. M., Clark, A. P., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2011). National income inequality predicts women’s preferences for masculinized faces better than health does. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 278, 810–812.

  • Butovskaya, M. L., Windhager, S., Karelin, D., Mezentseva, A., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2018). Associations of physical strength with facial shape in an African pastoralist society, the Maasai of northern Tanzania. PLoS One, 13(5), e0197738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, A. P., & Fisher, M. (2009). Individual differences in intrasexual competition. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 37–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, A. P., Park, J. H., Zurriaga, R., Klavina, L., & Massar, K. (2008). Height predicts jealousy differently for men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 133–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, S., & Astrom, K. (2004). Christmas-A cross-sectional study of UK academics suggests Santa Claus might be a professor. The Pharmaceutical Journal, 273, 897–899.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarkson, T. R., Sidari, M. J., Sains, R., Alexander, M., Harrison, M., Mefodeva, V., Pearson, S., Lee, A. J., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2020). A multivariate analysis of women’s mating strategies and sexual selection on men’s facial morphology. Royal Society Open Science, 7(1), 191209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, B. M., Nelson, N. L., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2019). Sexual selection, agonistic signalling, and the effect of beards on men’s anger displays. Psychological Science, 30, 728–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., ... & Tiddeman, B. P. (2006). Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner's masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 273, 1355–1360.

  • Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Tibubos, A. N., Zhang, E., Harmon-Jones, E., & Kasumovic, M. M. (2020). Violent video game play, gender, and trait aggression influence subjective fighting ability, perceptions of men’s toughness, and anger facial recognition. Computers in Human Behavior, 104, 106175.

  • Dixson, B. J., & Brooks, R. C. (2013). The role of facial hair in women’s perceptions of men's attractiveness, health, masculinity and parenting abilities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 236–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., & Rantala, M. J. (2016). The role of facial and body hair distribution in women’s judgments of men’s sexual attractiveness. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 877–889.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Beards augment perceptions of men’s aggressiveness, dominance and age, but not attractiveness. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 481–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, A. F., Dixson, B. J., & Anderson, M. J. (2005). Sexual selection and the evolution of visually conspicuous sexually dimorphic traits in male monkeys, apes, and human beings. Annual Review of Sex Research, 16, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Tam, J., & Awasthy, M. (2013). Do women’s preferences for men’s facial hair change with reproductive status? Behavioral Ecology, 24, 708–716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Ormsby, D. K., & Dixson, A. F. (2014). Eye-tracking women's preferences for men’s somatotypes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 73–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., Sulikowski, D., Gouda-Vossos, A., Rantala, M. J., & Brooks, R. C. (2016). The masculinity paradox: Facial masculinity and beardedness interact to determine women’s ratings of men’s facial attractiveness. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 29, 2311–2320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., Rantala, M. J., Melo, E. F., & Brooks, R. C. (2017a). Beards and the big city: displays of masculinity may be amplified under crowded conditions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 259–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., Little, A. C., Dixson, H. G., & Brooks, R. C. (2017b). Do prevailing environmental factors influence human preferences for facial morphology? Behavioral Ecology, 28, 1217–1227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., Lee, A. J., Sherlock, J. M., & Talamas, S. N. (2017c). Beneath the beard: do facial morphometrics influence the strength of judgments of men’s beardedness? Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 164–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., Sherlock, J. M., Cornwall, W., & Kasumovic, M. M. (2018a). Contest competition and men’s facial hair: beards may not provide advantages in combat. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39, 147–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Blake, K. R., Denson, T. F., Gooda-Vossos, A., O’Dean, S. M., Sulikowski, D., & Brooks, R. C. (2018b). The role of mating context and fecundability in women’s preferences for men’s facial masculinity and beardedness. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 93, 90–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Lee, A. J., Blake, K. R., Jasienska, G., & Marcinkowska, U. M. (2018c). Women's preferences for men’s beards show no relation to their ovarian cycle phase and sex hormone levels. Hormones and Behavior, 97, 137–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Rantala, M. J., & Brooks, R. C. (2019a). Cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for men’s body hair. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 5, 131–147.

  • Dixson, B. J. W., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Lee, A. J., & Nelson, N. L. (2019b). Mothers are sensitive to men's beards as a potential cue of paternal investment. Hormones and Behavior, 113, 55–66.

  • Durkee, P. K. (2019). Do the Maasai perceive weak walkers to be stronger and more attractive than strong walkers? A re-analysis of Fink et al. (2019). Biology Letters, 15, 20190240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, D. J. (2008). The evolution of animal weapons. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 39, 387–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M., Holbrook, C., & Gervais, M. M. (2014). Men’s physical strength moderates conceptualizations of prospective foes in two disparate societies. Human Nature, 25, 393–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19, 82–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink, B., Wübker, M., Ostner, J., Butovskaya, M. L., Mezentseva, A., Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., et al. (2017). Cross-cultural investigation of male gait perception in relation to physical strength and speed. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink, B., Butovskaya, M. L., & Shackelford, T. K. (2019). Assessment of physical strength from gait: data from the Maasai of Tanzania. Biology Letters, 15(3), 20180803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, A. C., O'Brien, D. T., White, D. D., & Wilson, D. S. (2010). Handgrip strength and socially dominant behavior in male adolescents. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 147470491000800207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geniole, S. N., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Facing our ancestors: judgments of aggression are consistent and related to the facial width-to-height ratio in men irrespective of beards. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 279–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geniole, S. N., Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Carré, J. M., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Evidence from meta-analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PLoS One, 10(7), e0132726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of public domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grueter, C. C., Isler, K., & Dixson, B. J. (2015). Are badges of status adaptive in large complex primate groups? Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 398–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hellmer, K., & Stenson, T. J. (2016). Contradicting data and comments on Oldmeadow and Dixson’s (2015) “the association between men’s sexist attitudes and facial hair”. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45(4), 783–784.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hellmer, K., Stenson, J. T., & Jylhä, K. M. (2018). What’s (not) underpinning ambivalent sexism? Revisiting the roles of ideology, religiosity, personality, demographics, and men's facial hair in explaining hostile and benevolent sexism. Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 29–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. K., Hunt, J., Welling, L. L., Cárdenas, R. A., Rotella, M. A., Wheatley, J. R., et al. (2013). Quantifying the strength and form of sexual selection on men’s traits. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 334–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzleitner, I. J., & Perrett, D. I. (2016). Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37, 217–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, J. D., McGue, M. K., & Iacono, W. G. (2015). Aggressive-antisocial boys develop into physically strong young men. Psychological Science, 26, 444–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janif, Z. J., Brooks, R. C., & Dixson, B. J. (2014). Negative frequency-dependent preferences and variation in male facial hair. Biology Letters, 10, 20130958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knussman, R., & Christiansen, K. (1988). Attributes of masculinity and androgen level. Homo., 39, 45–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kordsmeyer, T. L., Hunt, J., Puts, D. A., Ostner, J., & Penke, L. (2018). The relative importance of intra-and intersexual selection on human male sexually dimorphic traits. Evolution and Human Behavior.

  • Kordsmeyer, T. L., Freund, D., Pita, S. R., Jünger, J., & Penke, L. (2019). Further evidence that facial width-to-height ratio and global facial masculinity are not positively associated with testosterone levels. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1–14.

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13).

  • Lailvaux, S. P., Reaney, L. T., & Backwell, P. R. (2009). Dishonest signalling of fighting ability and multiple performance traits in the fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi. Functional Ecology, 359–366.

  • Lassek, W. D., & Gaulin, S. J. (2009). Costs and benefits of fat-free muscle mass in men: relationship to mating success, dietary requirements, and native immunity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(5), 322–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukaszewski, A. W., Simmons, Z. L., Anderson, C., & Roney, J. R. (2016). The role of physical formidability in human social status allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcinkowska, U. M., Rantala, M. J., Lee, A. J., Kozlov, M. V., Toivo, A., Cai, T. H., Contreras-Garduño, J., David, O. A., Kaminski, G., Li, N. P., Onyishi, I. E., Prasai, K., Pazhoohi, F., Prokop, P., Cardozo, S. L. R., Sydney, N., Taniguchi, H., Krams, I., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2019). Women’s preferences for men’s facial masculinity are strongest under favorable ecological conditions. Scientific Reports.

  • Marečková, K., Weinbrand, Z., Chakravarty, M. M., Lawrence, C., Aleong, R., Leonard, G., et al. (2011). Testosterone-mediated sex differences in the face shape during adolescence: subjective impressions and objective features. Hormones and Behavior, 60, 681–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2012). Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 1–23.

  • McCullough, E. L., Miller, C. W., & Emlen, D. J. (2016). Why sexually selected weapons are not ornaments. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31, 742–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, T., Lee, A. J., Sidari, M., Stower, R., Sherlock, J. M., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2017). Microbes and masculinity: does exposure to pathogenic cues alter women’s preferences for male facial masculinity and beardedness? PLoS One, 12(6), e0178206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., Gil-Burmann, C., Fink, B., & Turiegano, E. (2012). Physical strength, fighting ability, and aggressiveness in adolescents. American Journal of Human Biology, 24, 611–617.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muscarella, F., & Cunningham, M. R. (1996). The evolutionary significance and social perception of male pattern baldness and facial hair. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 99–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neave, N., & Shields, K. (2008). The effects of facial hair manipulation on female perceptions of attractiveness, masculinity, and dominance in male faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 373–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, N. L., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Lee, A. J., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2019). Children’s judgements of facial hair are influenced by biological development and experience. Evolution and Human Behavior, 113, 55–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J. J., & Feinberg, D. R. (2012). The influence of facial masculinity and voice pitch on jealousy and perceptions of intra-sexual rivalry. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 369–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldmeadow, J. A., & Dixson, B. J. (2016). The association between men’s sexist attitudes and facial hair. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 891–899.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 229–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM, Henzi SP, Castles DL, Akamatsu S. 1998 Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887.

  • Peters, M., Simmons, L. W., & Rhodes, G. (2008). Testosterone is associated with mating success but not attractiveness or masculinity in human males. Animal Behaviour, 76, 297–303.

  • Phalane, K. G., Tribe, C., Steel, H. C., Cholo, M. C., & Coetzee, V. (2017). Facial appearance reveals immunity in African men. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 7443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinto, N. S., Palaoro, A. V., & Peixoto, P. E. (2019). All by myself? Meta-analysis of animal contests shows stronger support for self than for mutual assessment models. Biological Reviews.

  • Polo, P., Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., Pita, M., Shackelford, T. K., & Fink, B. (2019). Testosterone dependent facial and body traits predict men's sociosexual attitudes and behaviors. American Journal of Human Biology, e23235.

  • Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 157–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., & Reno, P. L. (2015). Contest competition in men. The Handbook of evolutionary psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

  • R Core Team. (2013) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

  • Raine, J., Pisanski, K., Oleszkiewicz, A., Simner, J., & Reby, D. (2018). Human listeners can accurately judge strength and height relative to self from aggressive roars and speech. iScience, 4, 273–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Randall, V. A. (2008). Androgens and hair growth. Dermatologic Therapy, 21, 314–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rantala, M. J., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kivleniece, I., Kecko, S., & Krams, I. (2012). Evidence for the stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in humans. Nature Communications, 3, 694.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rantala, M. J., Coetzee, V., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Kecko, S., Krama, T., et al. (2013). Adiposity, compared with masculinity, serves as a more valid cue to immunocompetence in human mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1751), 20122495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reaney, L. T., Milner, R. N. C., Detto, T., & Backwell, P. R. Y. (2008). The effects of claw regeneration on territory ownership and mating success in the fiddler crab, Uca mjoebergi. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1473–1478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W, & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 186–201.

  • Rhodes, G., Chan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Simmons, L. W. (2003). Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270, S93–S95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G., Morley, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2013). Women can judge sexual unfaithfulness from unfamiliar men’s faces. Biology Letters, 9, 20120908.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rico-Guevara, A., & Hurme, K. J. (2019). Intrasexually selected weapons. Biological Reviews, 94, 60–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roosenboom, J., Indencleef, K., Lee, M. K., Hoskens, H., White, J. D., Liu, D., et al. (2018). SNPs associated with testosterone levels influence human facial morphology. Frontiers in Genetics, 9.

  • Rosenfield, K. A., Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P., & Puts, D. A. (2020). Sexual selection for low male voice pitch among Amazonian forager-horticulturists. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41, 3–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxton, T. K., Mackey, L. L., McCarty, K., & Neave, N. (2016). A lover or a fighter? Opposing sexual selection pressures on men’s vocal pitch and facial hair. Behavioral Ecology, 27, 512–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Boothroyd, L. G., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2013). Do men’s faces really signal heritable immunocompetence? Behavioral Ecology, 24, 579–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Josephson, S. C., Boyette, A. H., Cuthill, I. C., Fried, R. L., et al. (2014). Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 14388–14393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 276, 575–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Bryant, G., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., Krauss, A., & Gurven, M. (2010). Adaptations in humans for assessing physical strength from the voice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 3509–3518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Hone, L. S., & Pound, N. (2012). The importance of physical strength to human males. Human Nature, 23, 30–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherlock, J. M., Tegg, B., Sulikowski, D., & Dixson, B. J. (2017). Facial masculinity and beardedness determine men’s explicit, but not their implicit, responses to male dominance. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 3, 14–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singal, P., Bhatnagar, D. P., & Kaur, S. (2006). Anthropometric profile and development of facial hair in male athletes. Journal of Exercise Science and Physiotherapy, 2, 52–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snell Jr., W. E. (1989). Development and validation of the masculine behavior scale: a measure of behaviors stereotypically attributed to males vs. females. Sex Roles, 21, 749–767.

    Google Scholar 

  • Štěrbová, Z., Tureček, P., & Kleisner, K. (2019). She always steps in the same river: similarity among long-term partners in their demographic, physical, and personality characteristics. Frontiers in Psychology, 10.

  • Stower, R. E., Lee, A. J., McIntosh, T. L., Sidari, M. J., Sherlock, J. M., & Dixson, B. J. (2019). Mating strategies and the masculinity paradox: how relationship context, relationship status, and Sociosexuality shape women’s preferences for facial masculinity and beardedness. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 1–12.

  • Stulp, G., & Barrett, L. (2016). Evolutionary perspectives on human height variation. Biological Reviews, 91, 206–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrizzi, B. F., Brey, E., Shutts, K., & Beier, J. S. (2019). Children’s developing judgments about the physical manifestations of power. Developmental Psychology, 55, 793.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 131–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobin-Richards, M. H., Boxer, A. M., & Petersen, A. C. (1983). The psychological significance of pubertal change. In J. Brooks-Gunn & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty (pp. 127–154). Boston: Springer US.

    Google Scholar 

  • Třebický, V., Havlíček, J., Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., & Kleisner, K. (2013). Perceived aggressiveness predicts fighting performance in mixed-martial-arts fighters. Psychological Science, 24, 1664–1672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Třebický, V., Fialová, J., Kleisner, K., Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., & Havlíček, J. (2015). Further evidence for links between facial width-to-height ratio and fighting success: commentary on Zilioli et al.(2014). Aggressive Behavior, 41, 331–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Třebický, V., Fialová, J., Stella, D., Coufalová, K., Pavelka, R., Kleisner, K., et al. (2018). Predictors of fighting ability inferences based on faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentova, J. V., Varella, M., Bártová, K., Štěrbová, Z., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2017). Mate preferences and choices for facial and body hair in heterosexual women and homosexual men: effects of sex, population, homogamy, and imprinting-like effects. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 241–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Doorn, J., van den Bergh, D., Bohm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., … Wagenmakers, E. (2019, January 23). The JASP Guidelines for Conducting and Reporting a Bayesian Analysis. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/yqxfr

  • von Rueden, C. R., & Jaeggi, A. V. (2016). Men’s status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 10824–10829.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. D., Fraccaro, P. J., Smith, F. G., Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2010a). Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 943–947.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. D., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2010b). Individual differences in dominance perception: dominant men are less sensitive to facial cues of male dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 967–971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J., Gilani, S. Z., Shafait, F., Mian, A., Tan, D. W., Maybery, M. T., et al. (2015). Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20151351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S., James, R. S., Bywater, C., & Seebacher, F. (2009). Costs and benefits of increased weapon size differ between sexes of the slender crayfish, Cherax dispar. Journal of Experimental Biology, 212, 853–858.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23, 805–814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, B. B., & Candolin, U. (2005). How is female mate choice affected by male competition? Biological Reviews, 80, 559–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. R. (1986). Self-perceived masculinity between bearded and non-bearded males. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 769–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, E., Galbany, J., McFarlin, S. C., Ndayishimiye, E., Stoinski, T. S., & Robbins, M. M. (2019). Male body size, dominance rank and strategic use of aggression in a group-living mammal. Animal Behaviour, 151, 87–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaidi, A. A., White, J. D., Mattern, B. C., Liebowitz, C. R., Puts, D. A., Claes, P., & Shriver, M. D. (2019). Facial masculinity does not appear to be a condition-dependent male ornament and does not reflect MHC heterozygosity in humans, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences., 116, 1633–1638.

  • Zilioli, S., Sell, A. N., Stirrat, M., Jagore, J., Vickerman, W., & Watson, N. V. (2015). Face of a fighter: bizygomatic width as a cue of formidability. Aggressive Behavior, 41, 322–330.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a University of Queensland Postdoctoral Fellowship to BJWD.

Data Accessibility

Data are available at the Open Science Framework.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

BJWD, VM, HC, GS, and BF designed and conducted the study; SP and MJS collected the data; AJL, MJS, BJWD, VM, and TC analyzed the data; BJWD, VM, MJS, HC, BF, GS, TRC, SP, and AJL wrote the manuscript and commented and drafts. BJWD provided funding for the research.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barnaby J. W. Dixson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no competing interests.

Ethical Statement

Ethics clearance from the University of Queensland’s Behavioural and Social Sciences Ethical Review Committee and the School of Psychology’s Ethics Review Panel (Ethics Approval Number: 18-PSYCH-4G-13-JMC).

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mefodeva, V., Sidari, M.J., Chau, H. et al. Multivariate Intra-Sexual Selection on Men’s Perceptions of Male Facial Morphology. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 6, 143–169 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00128-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00128-2

Keywords

Navigation