Skip to main content
Log in

Education and the Gender Gaps in Health and Mortality

  • Published:
Demography

Abstract

The positive associations between education and health and survival are well established, but whether the strength of these associations depends on gender is not. Is the beneficial influence of education on survival and on self-rated health conditioned by gender in the same way, in opposite ways, or not at all? Because women are otherwise disadvantaged in socioeconomic resources that are inputs to health, their health and survival may depend more on education than will men’s. To test this hypothesis, we use data from the National Health Interview Survey-Linked Mortality Files (NHIS-LMF). We find that education’s beneficial influence on feeling healthy and on survival are conditional on gender, but in opposite ways. Education has a larger effect on women’s self-rated health than on men’s, but a larger effect on men’s mortality. To further examine the mortality results, we examine specific causes of death. We find that the conditional effect is largest for deaths from lung cancer, respiratory disease, stroke, homicide, suicide, and accidents. Because women report worse health but men’s mortality is higher, education closes the gender gap in both health and mortality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Hypotheses derived from resource substitution on other disadvantaged statuses, such as race or parental education, are also somewhat inconclusive. In support of resource substitution, recent evidence shows that personal education has a larger influence on health among persons whose parents were poorly educated than among those from more advantaged family backgrounds (Ross and Mirowsky 2011), but, counter to resource substitution theory, that education has a smaller impact on health among blacks than whites (Williams 1997).

  2. Ordered logistic regression is a proportional-odds model, in that it assumes the effect of a predictor on the log odds of reporting a higher category over all lower categories is the same across all possible higher-lower comparisons. This assumption of “parallel regression” was tested for education’s effect on self-rated health by running separate binary logistic regression models of all possible higher-lower categorical comparisons, and was found to be satisfactory.

  3. All coefficients noted as significant at p < .001 (t > 3.29) in fact have t values greater than 3.7 \( \sqrt {{\ln (n)}} \), which has been suggested as a conservative test of significance.

  4. The one exception is that compared with having a college degree, having a master’s degree or more does not continue to close the gender gap but rather re-opens it somewhat. Master’s degrees or higher do not benefit women’s health very much. However, caution should be taken in interpreting this because the education measure is truncated at master’s level and higher, and this category probably contains differences in the educational attainment of men and women, with the men more likely to have achieved an M.D., J.D., or Ph.D. and the women more likely to be in the master’s category. Other research has shown a leveling off in the health benefits of a master’s degree compared with a bachelor of arts degree, but a substantial health benefit to more advanced degrees (Ross and Mirowsky 1999), possibly because master’s degree fields such as social work or nursing (typically attained by women) do not translate into high-status jobs that pay well. These data do not allow us to make the distinction between master’s degrees and more-advanced degrees.

  5. Age equivalents equal the coefficient of interest divided by the coefficient associated with age. To take an example from Table 2 in the prediction of poor self-rated health, the coefficient associated with each year of schooling is –0.161 for men and −0.172 for women (−0.161 − [−0.011]); the coefficient associated with each year of age is 0.047 for men and 0.039 for women (0.047 – 0.008). For women: –0.172 / 0.039 = –4.410, and for men: −0.161 / 0.047 = −3.425. Thus, the difference in age equivalents between 12 years and 16 years of schooling is 17.64 for women and 13.7 for men, a difference of 3.94 years. Age equivalents provide a standard of comparison in the study of health and mortality because age is consistently associated with declines in health and increases in mortality (Mirowsky 2005).

  6. This speculative explanation refers to dangerous behaviors, not all risk factors. Some risk factors, such as a sedentary lifestyle or a high-fat diet, are not thought of as dangerous and destructive. Although a sedentary lifestyle, excess calories, and being overweight are major risk factors for heart disease, diabetes, stroke, and other fatal health problems, they are common and normative. (And men are not even at higher risk, given that they exercise more than women). In fact, this has been called the “default” American lifestyle (Mirowsky and Ross 2010). Future research on these ideas requires data with measures of health lifestyle, including dangerous behaviors—and, ideally, enough cases to examine specific causes of death. According to Pampel (2002), smoking is the behavior most destructive to health, accounting for a significant portion of men’s excess mortality and for the narrowing gender gap in mortality in recent years (as women’s cigarette use approaches men’s), so initial tests can be undertaken with recent years of the NHIS-LMF, which includes measures of smoking. However, we found conditional effects for homicide, suicide, and accidents, in addition to lung cancer and emphysema, which suggest that dangerous behaviors other than smoking may also be important. In fact, some of these dangerous behaviors influence mortality directly (from accidents, homicide, and to some extent suicide), not by way of poor health (Benjamins et al. 2004).

    Most causes of death, like heart disease, are indirectly influenced by health and could have counteracting effects that cancel each other in the following way. Men’s advantaged status may give them the latitude to engage in dangerous behaviors, but obviously men’s position isn’t all bad for health. Their better jobs and higher incomes, for example, benefit health. Thus, there could be counteracting effects of risky lifestyle and socioeconomic advantage, with interactions in opposite directions. When destructive lifestyle does not predominate, the counteracting interactions could balance, producing null or nonsignificant interactions of education and sex on mortality. While speculative, this is consistent with what we find for cancer (other than lung cancer), infectious disease, and, especially, heart disease.

References

  • Arber, S. (1997). Comparing inequalities in women’s and men’s health: Britain in the 90’s. Social Science & Medicine, 44, 773–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arber, S., & Ginn, J. (1993). Gender and inequalities in health in later life. Social Science & Medicine, 36, 33–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Avendano, M., Kunst, A. E., Huisman, M., Van Lenthe, F., & Bopp, M. (2004). Educational level and stroke mortality: A comparison of 10 European populations during the 1990s. Stroke, 35, 432–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassuk, S. S., Berkman, L. F., & Amick, B. C. (2002). Socioeconomic status and mortality among the elderly: Findings from four US communities. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155, 520–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamins, M. R., Hummer, R. A., Eberstein, I. W., & Nam, C. B. (2004). Self-reported health and adult mortality risk: An analysis of cause-specific mortality. Social Science & Medicine, 59, 1297–1306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, C. E., & Rieker, P. P. (2008). Gender and health: The effects of constrained choices and social policies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Case, A., & Paxson, C. (2005). Sex differences in morbidity and mortality. Demography, 42, 189–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, C. F. (2007). Great divides: The cultural, cognitive, and social bases of the global subordination of women. American Sociological Review, 72, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, N. D., & Weaver, C. N. (1981). The contribution of marital happiness to global happiness. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43, 161–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorman, B. K., & Read, J. G. (2006). Gender disparities in adult health: An examination of three measures of morbidity. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 47, 95–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huisman, M., Kunst, A. E., Bopp, M., Borgan, J. K., Borrell, C., Costa, G., & Mackenbach, J. P. (2005). Education inequalities in cause-specific mortality in middle-aged and older men and women in eight western European populations. Lancet, 365, 493–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummer, R. A., & Lariscy, J. (2011). Educational attainment and adult mortality. In R. G. Rogers & E. M. Crimmins (Eds.), International handbook of adult mortality (pp. 241–261). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, H. H., Wright, C. R., & Reed, J. S. (1976). The enduring effects of education. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kilbourne, B. S., England, P., Farkas, G., Beron, K., & Weir, D. (1994). Returns to skill, compensating differentials, and gender bias: Effects of occupational characteristics on the wages of white women and men. American Journal of Sociology, 100, 689–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingston, P. W., Hubbard, R., Lapp, B., Schroeder, P., & Wilson, J. (2003). Why education matters. Sociology of Education, 76, 53–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitagawa, E. M., & Hauser, P. M. (1973). Differential mortality in the United States: A study in socioeconomic epidemiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M., & Slomczynski, K. M. (1993). Social structure and self-direction. A comparative analysis of the United States and Poland. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (1995). “Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior (extra issue):80–94.

  • Lochner, K., Hummer, R. A., Bartee, S., Wheatcroft, G., & Cox, C. (2008). The public-use national health interview survey linked mortality files: Methods of reidentification risk avoidance and comparative analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 168, 336–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, S., Manor, O., & Power, C. (1999). Social inequalities in health: Are there gender differences? Social Science & Medicine, 48, 49–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J. (2005). Age at first birth, health and mortality. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46, 32–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Hu, P. N. (1996). Physical impairment and the diminishing effects of income. Social Forces, 74, 1073–1096.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1998). Education, personal control, lifestyle and health: A human capital hypotheses. Research on Aging, 20, 415–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2003). Education, social status, and health. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2005). Education, learned effectiveness, and health. London Review of Education, 3, 205–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2007). Life course trajectories of perceived control and their relationship to education. American Journal of Sociology, 112, 1339–1382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2010). Self-direction toward health: Overriding the default american lifestyle. In J. M. Suls, K. Davidson, & R. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of health psychology (pp. 235–250). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., Ross, C. E., & Reynolds, J. (2000). Social status and health status. In C. E. Bird, P. Conrad, & A. M. Fremont (Eds.), Handbook of medical sociology (pp. 47–67). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montez, J. K., Hayward, M. D., Brown, D. C., & Hummer, R. A. (2009). Why is the educational gradient in mortality steeper for men? Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 64, 625–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostrove, J. M., & Adler, N. E. (1998). The relationship of socioeconomic status, labor force participation, and health among men and women. Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 451–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pampel, F. C. (2002). Cigarette use and the narrowing sex differential in mortality. Population and Development Review, 28, 77–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pampel, F. C., & Rogers, R. G. (2004). Socioeconomic status, smoking, and health: A test of competing theories of cumulative advantage. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45, 306–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (1991). How college affects students. San Francisco, CA: Josey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regidor, E., Calle, M. E., Navarro, P., & Dominiquez, V. (2003). The size of educational differences in mortality from specific causes of death in men and women. European Journal of Epidemiology, 18, 395–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reskin, B., & Padavic, I. (1994). Women and men at work. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, J. R., & Ross, C. E. (1998). Social stratification and health: Education’s benefit beyond economic status and social origins. Social Problems, 45, 221–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Bird, C. E. (1994). Sex stratification and health lifestyle: Consequences for men’s and women’s perceived health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 161–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1999). Refining the association between education and health: Effects of quantity, credential, and selectivity. Demography, 36, 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2002). Age and the gender gap in the sense of personal control. Social Psychology Quarterly, 65, 125–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2006). Sex differences in the effect of education on depression: Resource multiplication or resource substitution? Social Science & Medicine, 63, 1400–1413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2010a). Gender and the health benefits of education. The Sociological Quarterly, 51, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2010b). Why education is key to socioeconomic differentials in health. In C. E. Bird, P. Conrad, A. M. Fremont, & S. Timmermans (Eds.), Handbook of medical sociology (6th ed., pp. 33–51). Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2011). The interaction of personal and parental education on health. Social Science & Medicine, 72, 591–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnittker, J. (2004). Education and the changing shape of the income gradient in health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45, 286–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sen, A. (1997). Human capital and human capability. World Development, 25, 1959–1961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spaeth, J. L. (1976). Cognitive complexity: A dimension underlying the socioeconomic achievement process. In W. H. Sewell, R. M. Hauser, & D. L. Featherman (Eds.), Schooling and achievement in American society (pp. 103–131). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steenland, K. S., Henley, J., & Thun, M. J. (2002). All-cause and cause-specific death rates by educational status for two million people in two American Cancer Society cohorts, 1959–1996. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156, 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurston, R. C., Kubzansky, L. D., Kawachi, I., & Berkman, L. F. (2005). Is the association between socioeconomic position and coronary heart disease stronger in women than in men? American Journal of Epidemiology, 162(1), 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). The statistical abstract of the United States 2002. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umberson, D. (1992). Gender, marital status and the social control of health behavior. Social Science & Medicine, 34, 907–917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verbrugge, L. (1985). Gender and health: An update on hypotheses and evidence. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 26, 156–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verbrugge, L. (1989). The twain meet: Empirical explanations of sex differences in health and mortality. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30, 282–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldron. (1997). Changing gender roles and gender differences in health behavior. In D. Gochman (Ed.), Handbook of health behavior research (pp. 303–328). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wamala, S. P., Lynch, J., & Kaplan, G. A. (2001). Women’s exposure to early ad later life socioeconomic disadvantage and coronary heart disease risk: The Stockholm Female Coronary Risk Study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30, 275–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheaton, B. (1980). The sociogenesis of psychological disorder: An attributional theory. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 100–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. R. (1990). Socioeconomic differentials in health: A review and redirection. Social Psychology Quarterly, 53, 81–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. R. (1997). Race and health: Basic questions, emerging directions. Annals of Epidemiology, 7, 322–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wray, L. A., Regula Herzog, A., Willis, R. J., & Wallace, R. B. (1998). The impact of education and heart attack on smoking cessation among middle-aged adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 4, 271–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajacova, A. (2006). Education, gender, and mortality: Does schooling have the same effect on mortality for men and women in the US? Social Science & Medicine, 63, 2176–2190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajacova, A., & Hummer, R. A. (2009). Gender differences in education effects on all-cause mortality for white and black adults in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 69, 529–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (RO1-HD 053696) to Robert Hummer, and a grant from the National Institute on Aging to Catherine Ross (RO1-AG035268).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Catherine E. Ross.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ross, C.E., Masters, R.K. & Hummer, R.A. Education and the Gender Gaps in Health and Mortality. Demography 49, 1157–1183 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-012-0130-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-012-0130-z

Keywords

Navigation