Skip to main content
Log in

Anchoring Social Symbol Grounding in Children’s Interactions

  • Research Project
  • Published:
KI - Künstliche Intelligenz Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this article, we will discuss how computational social symbol grounding (i.e. how shared sets of symbols are grounded in multi-agent models) can be used to study children’s acquisition of word-meaning mappings. In order to use multi-agent modelling as a reliable tool to study human language acquisition, we argue that the simulations need to be anchored in observations of social interactions that children encounter “in the wild” and in different cultures. We discuss what aspects of such social interactions and cognitive mechanisms can and should be modelled, as well as how we intend to anchor this model to corpora containing features of children’s social behaviour as observed “in the wild” to mimic children’s (social) environment as reliably as possible. In addition, we discuss some challenges that need to be solved in order to construct the computational model. The resulting SCAFFOLD model will provide a benchmark for investigating socio-cognitive mechanisms of human social symbol grounding using computer simulations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In contrast to earlier notions of anchoring (e.g., [3, 9]), we will use this notion to speak about connecting or grounding computational studies in empirical data. We use this term to distinguish from (symbol) grounding.

  2. Cultural And Social Aspects of Multimodal Interactions in Language Acquisition.

References

  1. Bakeman R, Adamson L (1984) Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Dev 55:1278–1289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Bangerter A, Oppenheimer D (2006) Accuracy in detecting referents of pointing gestures unaccompanied by language. Gesture 6(1):85–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Barwise J, Perry J (1983) Situations and attitudes. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  4. Blythe R, Smith K, Smith A (2010) Learning times for large lexicons through cross-situational learning. Cogn Sci 34(4):620–642

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Brown P (2011) The cultural organization of attention. In: The handbook of language socialization, pp 29–55

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  6. Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M, Butterworth G, Moore C (1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the society for research in child development, vol 63(4)

    Google Scholar 

  7. Chouinard MM, Clark EV (2003) Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. J Child Lang 30(3):637–669

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Clark HH (1996) Using language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  9. Coradeschi S, Saffiotti A (2003) An introduction to the anchoring problem. Robot Auton Syst 43(2):85–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Thal D, Bates E, Hartung J, Pethick S, Reilly J (1993) The MacArthur communicative development inventories: user’s guide and technical manual. Singular Publishing Group, San Diego

    Google Scholar 

  11. Frank M, Goodman N, Tenenbaum J (2009) Using speakers’ referential intentions to model early cross-situational word learning. Psychol Sci 20(5):578–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Gleitman L (1990) The structural sources of verb meanings. Lang Acquis 1:3–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42:335–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hart B, Risley T (1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  15. Keller H, Völker S, Yovsi R (2005) Conceptions of parenting in different cultural communities: the case of West African NSO and northern German women. Soc Dev 14(1):158–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kwisthout J, Vogt P, Haselager P, Dijkstra T (2008) Joint attention and language evolution. Connect Sci 20:155–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Markman E, Hutchinson J (1984) Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: taxonomic versus thematic relations. Cogn Psychol 16(1):1–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Markman EM, Wachtel GF (1988) Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meaning of words. Cogn Psychol 20:121–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Mastin JD, Vogt P (submitted) Analyzing infant engagement: filling the gaps in research approaches

  20. Mastin JD, Vogt P (submitted) Correlations between joint engagement and vocabulary development: a longitudinal, observational study of Mozambican infants from 1;1 to 2;1

  21. Peirce CS (1931–1958) Collected papers, vols I–VIII. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  22. Quine WVO (1960) Word and object. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  23. Rosch E (1978) Principles of categorization. In: Rosch E, Lloyd BB (eds) Cognition and categorization. Lawrence Erlbaum Ass, Hilldate

    Google Scholar 

  24. Rowe M, Goldin-Meadow S (2009) Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323(5916):951–953

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Schieffelin B, Ochs E (1989) Language socialization across cultures. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  26. Siskind JM (1996) A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings. Cognition 61:39–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Smith K, Smith A, Blythe R (2011) Cross-situational learning: an experimental study of word-learning mechanisms. Cogn Sci 35(3):480–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Smith LB, Yu C (2008) Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106(3):1558–1568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Snedeker J (2009) Word learning. In: Encyclopedia of neuroscience, pp 503–508

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  30. Soja NN, Carey S, Spelke ES (1991) Ontological categories guide young children’s inductions of word meanings: object terms and substance terms. Cognition 38:179–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Steels L (2003) Evolving grounded communication for robots. Trends Cogn Sci 7(7):308–312. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00129-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28(05):675–691

    Google Scholar 

  33. Vogt P (2002) The physical symbol grounding problem. Cogn Syst Res 3(3):429–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Vogt P (2012) Exploring the robustness of cross-situational learning under Zipfian distributions. Cogn Sci 36:726–739

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Vogt P, de Boer B (2010) Language evolution: computer models for empirical data. Adapt Behav 18(1):5–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Vogt P, Divina F (2007) Social symbol grounding and language evolution. Interact Stud 8(1):31–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Vogt P, Haasdijk E (2010) Modelling social learning of language and skills. Artif Life 16(4):289–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Vogt P, Lieven E (2010) Verifying theories of language acquisition using computer models of language evolution. Adapt Behav 18:21–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Vogt P, Mastin JD (submitted) Child-directed speech and co-speech gestures’ correlations with vocabulary development in rural and urban Mozambique

  40. Vygotsky L (1978) Mind in society. Harvard University Press, Harvard

    Google Scholar 

  41. Yu C, Ballard D (2007) A unified model of early word learning: integrating statistical and social cues. Neurocomputing 70(13–15):2149–2165

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Zukow-Goldring P (1996) Sensitive caregiving fosters the comprehension of speech: when gestures speak louder than words. Early Dev Parent 5(4):195–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research presented in this article is funded by a Vidi grant awarded to the first author by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO, grant no. 276-70-018). The authors are indebted to Wona Sanana and the Associação Communitario Ambiente de Mafalala for their assistance in recruiting the Mozambican participants, all Mozambican and Dutch research assistants for helping to collect and annotate the data, and all Mozambican and Dutch participants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Vogt.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Vogt, P., Mastin, J.D. Anchoring Social Symbol Grounding in Children’s Interactions. Künstl Intell 27, 145–151 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13218-013-0243-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13218-013-0243-6

Keywords

Navigation