Introduction

The influence of culture on sexuality and the phenomenon of migration have led to the development of studies that relate sexual attitudes to ethnocultural origin. Sexuality is conceived differently in each culture, giving it different meanings (Heinemann et al., 2016). These different ways of conceiving sexuality lead to differences in sexual attitudes between diverse ethnocultural groups.

Several investigations have found differences in sexual attitudes between diverse ethnocultural groups (e.g., Ahrold & Meston, 2010). In Canada, Meston et al. (1998) found that Asians have more conservative sexual attitudes than Latinos and Euro-Americans. In the USA, Padilla and Grady (1987) found that Mexican–American students had more conservative sexual attitudes than their Anglo peers. In this line, in that country, later, Eisenman and Dantzker (2006) found that Latinos have more conservative sexual attitudes than Euro-Americans. Ahrold and Meston (2010) found that Euro-Americans have more liberal sexual attitudes towards gender role traditionality than Hispanics and Asians in the USA. Moreover, they also found that Asians have more conservative sexual attitudes towards homosexuality and casual sex than Hispanics and Euro-Americans. In a review of literature, Fugère et al. (2008) found that African Americans have the most permissive sexual attitudes, followed by White Americans, then by Hispanic Americans and Asian Americans. In general, the studies show that minority groups (Asians and Latinos/Hispanics) have more conservative sexual attitudes than the majority group (Euro-Americans).

Although each ethnocultural group has its own sexual attitudes, in multicultural contexts, the contact with people from other ethnocultural groups could modify the sexual attitudes. That is, people from a group (e.g., minority group) by interacting with people from another culture (e.g., majority group) could modify their sexual attitudes. In addition, more and more people are born and grow up in a cultural context that is different from that of their own ethnocultural group, learning customs that are different from those learned by their ancestors. Therefore, in addition to the ethnocultural origin, the acculturation process could be related to sexual attitudes.

Acculturation

Acculturation is the process of cultural and psychological change that follows when there is continuous contact between people from diverse ethnocultural groups (Berry, 2003). This process implies changes in identity, attitudes, values, and behaviors in the groups involved. Although minority groups are usually the most affected (Berry, 1980, 2005).

Acculturation has been understood in multiple ways according to the different models that have been proposed (e.g., Berry, 1980; Gordon, 1964). Acculturation models can be classified mainly into two types: unidimensional model and bidimensional model (Ryder et al., 2000). In the unidimensional approach to acculturation, immigrants are placed on a continuum of identities ranging from exclusively heritage culture to exclusively mainstream culture (Gordon, 1964). In the middle of the continuum is biculturalism, where the immigrants maintain aspects of their culture and adopt those of the host culture. According to this model, the penetration into the mainstream culture (assimilation) is necessary to be successful in the host society (Ryder et al., 2000). For this reason, when this model is followed, “high acculturation” implies high adoption of the aspects of the host culture and low maintenance of those of the home culture.

In contrast, the bidimensional approach considers acculturation as a process in which both heritage and mainstream culture are two independent dimensions (Berry, 1980). Heritage acculturation refers to the degree to which ethnocultural groups maintain aspects (attitudes, values, behaviors, etc.) of their home cultural (culture of origin), while mainstream acculturation refers to the degree to which ethnocultural groups adopt aspects of the host culture (Berry, 2005).

Because culture plays an important role in sexuality (sexuality is constituted within a cultural context) and in multicultural contexts the acculturation process could be related to sexual attitudes, the study of the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes is relevant. For instance, migrants are exposed to different forms of sexual relationships in their host countries. Therefore, their sexual attitudes undergo a dynamic process of adjustment and adaptation.

Sexual Attitudes

Permissiveness, liberal and conservative orientation, and erotophilia and erotophobia are concepts used when sexual attitudes are measured (Blanc & Rojas, 2017). Sexual permissiveness could be defined as “acceptance or endorsement of sexual behaviors within relationships characterized by relatively lower levels of commitment” (Taylor, 2005, p. 131). Sexual attitudes are also defined in terms of a liberal or conservative orientation which is conceived as a bipolar continuum. At the liberal end of this continuum are those people who feel that the expression of human sexuality should be open, free, and unrestrained. At the conservative orientation end are those who feel that sexual expression should be regulated and constrained (Hudson et al., 1983). The erotophobia-erotophilia construct is also conceived as a bipolar continuum. Erotophilia refers to a tendency to exhibit positive emotions and evaluations toward sexual cues, while erotophobia implies a tendency to exhibit negative emotions and evaluations toward sexual cues (Fisher et al., 1988).

The role of the sexual attitudes in sexuality, especially in sexual health and sexual activity, has been demonstrated in the scientific literature. With regard to sexual health, studies have shown that sexual attitudes are related to sexual functioning (e.g., Carvalho et al., 2013) and risky sexual behaviors (e.g., Sanders et al., 2006). On the one hand, people with more positive or liberal sexual attitudes have greater sexual desire (Blanc et al., 2017; Dosch et al., 2016), greater sexual assertiveness (Blanc et al., 2017), and they are more sexually satisfied (Hurlbert et al., 1993) than people with more negative or conservatism sexual attitudes. On the other hand, people with more positive sexual attitudes are more likely to consistently use contraceptive methods than those with more negative sexual attitudes (Hynie & Lydon, 1996; Sanders et al., 2006) or extremely positive (Hynie & Lydon, 1996).

With respect to sexual activity, studies have also shown that sexual attitudes are related to the number of different sexual behaviors performed (e.g., Blanc et al., 2018; Murray-Swank et al., 2005), the frequency of sexual behaviors (e.g., García-Vega et al., 2017; Lemer et al., 2013), and the number of sexual partners (e.g., Luquis et al., 2015; Rinehart et al., 2014). Specifically, people with more positive or liberal sexual attitudes perform a greater number of different sexual behaviors; they perform them more frequently and have a greater number of different sexual partners than people with more negative or conservatism sexual attitudes.

Since sexual attitudes are related to sexual health (sexual functioning and risky sexual behaviors) and sexual activity, it is relevant to know the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. If there is a relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes, the acculturation process should indirectly influence sexual health and sexual activity (e.g., Brotto et al., 2012). In other word, sexual attitudes should mediate the relationship between the acculturation process and sexual health (and sexual activity). For all the above, the aim of the current study was to perform a systematic review of the studies that have examined the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. The purpose was to know the studies that have been carried out on the subject (years and author/s), the characteristics of the samples, the quantitative measures used (acculturation and sexual attitude measures), and the main results on the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes.

Method

Search Strategy

A systematic literature review was conducted to identify studies that examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. This systematic review was conducted using the PRISMA strategy. The PRISMA Statement consists of a four-phase flow diagram (Fig. 1) and of a 27-ítem checklist (Moher et al., 2015). The review was carried out in the ProQuest and Scopus databases with the following search criteria:

  • ProQuest. Terms: sexual* AND attitude* AND acculturation; In: Anywhere except full text; Language: English and Spanish; Source type: Scholarly Journal; Document type: Article.

  • Scopus. Terms: sexual* AND attitude* AND acculturation; In: Title, abstract and keywords; Language: English and Spanish; Source type: Journal; Document type: Article.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram of the selection process

There was no time restriction applied to the literature search, and the search took place in October 2020.

Selection Criteria

Studies were included if they met the following criteria:

  1. 1.

    Quantitative studies. Studies that did not use a quantitative methodology were excluded. Only quantitative studies were included to allow greater comparability between the results of the studies.

  2. 2.

    Acculturation measures. Studies that used proxy indicators of acculturation were also included.

  3. 3.

    Sexual attitude measures. Studies that did not include sexual attitude measures were excluded.

  4. 4.

    General sexual attitude measures. Studies that measured attitude toward a specific aspect of sexuality (e.g., attitudes towards condoms) were excluded. The reason was because there are numerous measures that evaluate attitudes toward specific aspects of sexuality, and it is not possible to examine their relationship with the acculturation process in a single study.

  5. 5.

    Relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. Studies that, although they included acculturation and sexual attitude measures, did not examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes were excluded.

Data Extraction

The information extracted from the studies were as follows:

  • Author(s) and year of publication of the study.

  • Sample characteristics: sample size, gender, ethnocultural origin, and country where they were recruited.

  • Acculturation measure and reliability.

  • Sexual attitude measure and reliability.

  • Results that examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes.

Results

Study Selection

Figure 1 provides a flow diagram of the selection process. A total of 544 studies (399 in ProQuest and 145 in Scopus) were found. Of the total, 122 were duplicate studies. Therefore, 422 were non-duplicate studies. A total of 25 studies met the inclusion criteria. Tables 1, 2, 3, and 4 show the information extracted from the studies. The studies were classified according to the minority ethnocultural origin. Moreover, they were arranged chronologically by year of publication.

Table 1 Studies that examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes in Asians
Table 2 Studies that examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes in Latinos/Hispanics
Table 3 Studies that examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes in Iranian women
Table 4 Studies that examine the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes in different minority ethnocultural groups

Sample Characteristics

Of the 25 studies of this review, 15 were conducted in the USA, 9 studies in Canada, and one study in both country (USA and Canada). All studies were focused on minority ethnocultural groups. Regarding the origin of the minority ethnocultural groups, 12 studies included only Asians (Table 1), 7 studies included only Latinos/Hispanics (Table 2), and 2 studies included only Iranian women (Table 3). A total of 4 studies included different minority ethnocultural groups (Table 4). With respect to gender, 8 studies included only female and 4 studies only male. A total of 13 studies included only students.

Acculturation Measures

Acculturation measures in the studies were very diverse. Some studies included proxy indicators of acculturation such as generation (Abramson & Imai-Marquez 1982), length of residency in the country (Kennedy & Gorzalka, 2002; Meston et al., 1998), and language preference (Edwards et al., 2008; Jiménez et al., 2002).

Of the total number of studies that used measures of acculturation, 9 studies used the Vancouver Index of Acculturation (VIA; Ryder et al., 2000), 5 studies the Scale of Acculturation for Mexican-Americans (Cuellar et al., 1980) or any of its adaptations (e.g., ARSMA-II; Cuellar et al., 1995), one study the Marmot’s Acculturation Index (Marmot, 1975), one study the General Ethnicity Questionnaire (GEQ; Tsai et al., 2000), and one study indicators of acculturation preferences in the most private relative acculturation extended model (RAEM) areas (Navas & Rojas, 2010) and indicators of acculturation preferences and strategies in the sexual area (Blanc & Rojas, 2020). The VIA, the GEQ, and the RAEM indicators are bidimensional measures of acculturation. Therefore, they consider acculturation as a process in which both heritage and mainstream culture are two independent dimensions. However, the Marmot’s Acculturation Index is a unidimensional measure of acculturation. In the study where the GEQ was used (Benuto & Meana, 2008), only the version that evaluate the mainstream acculturation was included. The Scale of Acculturation for Mexican-Americans was elaborated as a unidimensional measure of acculturation (Cuellar et al., 1980). However, later, this measure was modified (Cuellar et al., 1995) and both heritage and mainstream dimensions were included. Although in some studies (Abdolsalehi-Najafi & Beckman, 2013; Castañeda, 2017), a linear acculturation score is obtained by subtracting the mean Mexican Orientation subscale score from the mean Anglo Orientation subscale score. In the proxy indicators and unidimensional measures, acculturation refers to mainstream acculturation.

A total of 15 studies provided the reliability of the acculturation measure using Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency coefficient. This coefficient ranged from 0.73 to 0.93.

Sexual Attitude Measures

Sexual attitude measures in the studies were also very diverse. The most widely used measure (in 8 studies) was the Sexual Attitude Subscale of the Derogatis Sexual Functioning Inventory (DSFI; Derogatis, 1978). The Mosher Forced-Choice Guilt Inventory or one of its adaptations (Mosher, 1966, 1988) was also used in 4 studies. The Sexual Attitudes Scale (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1987) and its short version (Hendrick et al., 2006) and the Sexual Comfort Scale (Marín & Marín, 1991) were used in 2 studies. The rest of the measures were only used in one study. A total of 12 studies provided the reliability of the sexual attitude measure, in most studies using Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency coefficient. This coefficient ranged from 0.71 to 0.96.

Results: Relationship Between Acculturation and Sexual Attitudes

Studies Where the Relationship Was Not Statistically Significant

Only in three of the studies (Abdolsalehi-Najafi & Beckman, 2013; Castañeda, 2017; Deardorff et al., 2008), a statistically significant relationship was not found between acculturation and sexual attitudes.

The results of the studies, differentiating between minority ethnocultural groups and types of acculturation measure, are summarized as follows:

Iranians

Bidimensional measure (Abdolsalehi-Najafi & Beckman, 2013). The correlation between acculturation and sex guilt was no significant when the education and religion were controlled.

Latinos/Hispanics

Proxy indicator (Deardorff et al., 2008). Language use was no related to sexual comfort.

Bidimensional measure (Castañeda, 2017). The correlation between acculturation and sexual permissiveness was no significant.

Studies Where the Relationship Was Statistically Significant

Asians

Proxy Indicators (Abramson & Imani-Marquez, 1982; Brotto et al., 2005, 2007; Dang et al., 2019; Kennedy & Gorzalka, 2002; Meston et al., 1998). Older generations, immigrated during or after 1987 and arrived in Canada age 13 or older had more sex guilt or more negative sexual attitudes than the younger generations, Asians born in Canada, immigrated to Canada before 1987, or arrived before the age of 13. In general, years in Canada were positively correlated to liberal sexual attitudes and negatively correlated to conservative sexual attitudes, although these relationships were not found in all studies

Unidimensional Measures (Huang & Uba, 1992). Acculturation was positively related to permissive sexual attitudes. More acculturated people accepted with less affection a greater variety and types of sexual behaviors than lees acculturated people.

Bidimensional Measures (Arhold & Meston, 2010; Blanc & Rojas, 2020; Brotto et al., 2005, 20072012; Dang et al., 2019; Guo, 2019; Morton & Gorzalka, 2013; Woo et al., 2011, 2012). In general, mainstream acculturation was positively related to liberal sexual attitudes and negatively related to sex guilt and dysfunctional sexual beliefs. In some studies, heritage acculturation was positively related to conservatism or negative sexual attitudes. Although heritage acculturation was also negatively correlated with conservative sexual attitudes and positively correlated with liberal sexual attitudes, heritage acculturation was not related to sex guilt. Interaction effects between mainstream and heritage acculturation, and between mainstream acculturation and gender on sexual attitudes, were also found.

Latinos/Hispanics

Proxy Indicators (Edwards et al., 2008; Jiménez et al., 2002). Latinas who were born in the USA and spoke English in the home were most likely to agree that their parents would want them to use protection if they had sex. Interaction effect of language preference and sexual attitudes on sexual behaviors was also found. Unassimilated adolescents (preferred to be interviewed in Spanish) with traditional attitudes on sexuality were more likely to have had fewer sexual partners in the past 12 months and in their lifetime, and to have had a later age at coital debut.

Unidimensional Measures (Amaro, 1988; Contreras et al., 1996; Marín et al., 1997). Acculturation was positively related to liberal, idealistic sexual attitudes and sexual comfort.

Bidimensional Measures (Arhold & Meston, 2010; Blanc & Rojas, 2020). In general, mainstream acculturation was positively related to liberal or positive sexual attitudes, whereas heritage acculturation was positively related to conservatism or negative sexual attitudes. Interaction effects between mainstream and heritage acculturation, and between heritage acculturation and gender on sexual attitudes, were also found.

Iranians

Unidimensional Measures (Hanassab, 1991). Acculturation was positively related to premarital sexual attitudes. More acculturated people have more liberal attitudes toward sex than less acculturated people.

Africans

Bidimensional Measures (Blanc & Rojas, 2020). In general, heritage acculturation was negatively related to positive sexual attitudes. Interaction effects between heritage acculturation and gender, and between mainstream acculturation and gender on sexual attitudes, were also found.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to perform a systematic review of the studies that have examined the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. Although several studies have found that each ethnocultural group has its own sexual attitudes (e.g., Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Fugerè et al., 2008), in multicultural contexts, the contact with people from other ethnocultural groups could modify the sexual attitudes or learn different sexual attitudes from those of the own group. In general, the investigations show that minority groups have more conservative sexual attitudes than the majority group (e.g., Ahrold & Meston, 2010). However, the process of acculturation can lead to changes in sexual attitudes of minority groups after adopting the culture of the majority group.

In this review, the studies have been classified according to the minority ethnocultural origin. Also, they have been arranged chronologically by year of publication. The oldest study is by Abramson and Imai-Marquez (1982) who included Japanese-Americans. The most recent study is by Blanc and Rojas (2020) who included different ethnocultural groups. The studies have been conducted in the USA and/or in Canada. The most evaluated ethnocultural group has been Asians.

In general, an evolution to acculturation measures has been observed. In older studies, proxy indicators such as length of residency in the country (Meston et al., 1998) and unidimensional measures of acculturation were used. In this way, only a continuum of identities ranging from exclusively heritage culture to exclusively mainstream culture was considered (Gordon, 1964). In these studies, high acculturation refers to high mainstream acculturation. They do not differentiate between heritage and mainstream dimensions. However, the most recent studies (e.g., Arhold & Meston, 2010) use bidimensional measures of acculturation and differentiate between heritage and mainstream dimensions. This shows how the way of conceptualizing the acculturation process has changed. The results of the reviewed studies support that bidimensional measures are more useful than unidimensional measures and proxy indicators. On the one hand, the bidimensional measures allow to detect that while in some studies (e.g., Brotto et al., 2007) there is a significant relationship between mainstream culture and sexual attitudes, no relationship is found between heritage culture and sexual attitudes. On the other hand, the bidimensional measures allow the detection of interaction effects (moderation effects) between mainstream and heritage acculturation in sexual attitudes. For instance, higher heritage acculturation predicted more conservative or less permissive attitudes (Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Brotto et al., 2005) only when mainstream acculturation is low.

In addition, some reviewed studies (e.g., Brotto et al., 2005) also show that bidimensional measures better predict sexual attitudes than proxy indicators. Therefore, unidimensional measures and proxy indicators may be weaker measures than bidimensional measures.

This systematic literature review demonstrates that there is relationship between the acculturation process and sexual attitudes. Only in three of the 25 studies (Abdolsalehi-Najafi & Beckman, 2013; Castañeda, 2017; Deardorff et al., 2008), there was not a significant relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. This could be due to the acculturation measures used. One of the studies (Deardorff et al., 2008) used a proxy indicator (language), and although two of the studies (Abdolsalehi-Najafi & Beckman, 2013; Castañeda, 2017) used a bidimensional measure, a linear acculturation score was obtained by subtracting the mean heritage orientation subscale score from the mean mainstream orientation subscale score.

In general, results show that acculturation (mainstream acculturation) is positively related to liberal/positive/permissive sexual attitudes when unidimensional measures (or proxy indicators) of acculturation are used. Results also show that mainstream acculturation is positively related to liberal or positive sexual attitudes when bidimensional measures of acculturation are used. This demonstrates that people from different ethnocultural groups with high mainstream acculturation have more similar sexual attitudes to those of the majority group than to those of their own ethnocultural group. As already noted in other studies (Dang et al., 2017, 2019), one possible explanation is that mainstream culture (e.g., Canadian culture), unlike heritage culture (Chinese culture), includes liberalizing and sex-positive sources of information and outreach. Therefore, Chinese people in Canada who adopt more aspects (attitudes, values, behaviors, etc.) of the host culture would be exposed to more of this material and ultimately become more confident, aware, and knowledgeable about their own sexuality. This would lead to more liberal sexual attitudes.

In general, the relationships between mainstream acculturation and sexual attitudes are not very different across the diversity of acculturation measures. That is to say, these relationships point in the same direction. However, the relationship between heritage acculturation and sexual attitudes is not that strong and it does not point in the same direction in the reviewed studies. For instance, Brotto et al. (2007) did not find relationship between heritage acculturation and sexual attitudes. Ahrold and Meston (2010) found that heritage acculturation was positively correlated with conservative sexual attitudes and negatively correlated with liberal sexual attitudes. Contrary, Dang et al. (2019) found that heritage acculturation was negatively correlated with conservative sexual attitudes and positively correlated with liberal sexual attitudes. The discrepancy in these results may be due to changes over time about sexuality. For instance, the East Asian community has seen large changes and the discourse regarding sexuality has also changed in China and other East Asian polities during the time (Ho et al., 2018). The sexual attitudes of minority groups seem to be more liberal than for past generations, and this could contribute to some of the discrepancy in the results.

Some studies also found relevant interaction (moderation) and mediation effects. Interaction effect between mainstream and heritage dimensions on sexual attitudes was found in different studies (Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Brotto et al., 2005; Guo, 2019). Brotto et al. (2005) found that women with low heritage acculturation had increasingly liberal sexual attitudes with increasingly mainstream acculturation. However, women with high heritage acculturation did not have such an increase. Ahrold and Meston (2010) found that the relationship between heritage acculturation and conservatism sexual attitudes was stronger at lower levels of mainstream acculturation. Guo (2019) found that the relationship between mainstream acculturation and liberal sexual attitudes was stronger at higher levels of heritage acculturation. Therefore, mainstream dimension moderates the relationship between heritage dimension and sexual attitudes, and heritage dimension moderates the relationship between mainstream dimension and sexual attitudes. The discrepancy in the interaction effects may be also due to changes over time about sexuality. For this reason, in the study of Guo (2019), an integration strategy (maintenance of the home culture and adoption of the host culture) predicts liberal sexual attitudes. However, in previous studies (Brotto et al., 2005), an assimilation strategy (only adoption of the host culture) predicts liberal sexual attitudes. In some minority ethnocultural groups, Blanc and Rojas (2020) also found interaction effects between heritage dimension and gender, and between mainstream dimension and gender on sexual attitudes. They found that at high levels of heritage acculturation, there are significant differences in sexual attitudes between male and female (having male more positive attitudes), but not in low levels of heritage acculturation. They also found that at low levels of mainstream acculturation, there are significant differences in sexual attitudes between male and female (having male more positive attitudes), but not in high levels of mainstream acculturation. That is, acculturation (heritage and mainstream dimensions) moderates the relation between gender and sexual attitudes. The result could be consistent with the social structure theory (Eagly & Wood, 1999). Social structural theory proposes that gender differences in sexuality are a result of gender differences in power. Therefore, societies with a large gender difference in power are expected to have greater gender differences in sexuality than more egalitarian societies.

It could be that the mainstream culture is more egalitarian than those of the minority groups. Moreover, acculturation is also related to sexual attitudes in a gender, while in the other, the relationship is not significant (e.g., Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Blanc & Rojas, 2020). Therefore, gender also moderates the relation between acculturation and sexual attitudes.

Some studies (e.g., Ahrold & Meston, 2010; Blanc & Rojas, 2020) also demonstrate that the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes is different depending on the ethnocultural group. For example, Ahrold and Meston (2010) found that relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes is stronger in Asians than in Hispanics. Therefore, the ethnocultural origin also moderates the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes. This may be due to differences in sexual attitudes between the majority group and minority groups. The relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes could be stronger in those minority groups that have different sexual attitudes to those of the majority group than those that have similar sexual attitudes. These findings are consistent with the findings of Du and Li (2015). They found in a systematic review regarding the relationship between acculturation and HIV-related sexual behaviors the following results: gender moderated the relationship between acculturation and sexual behaviors, and ethnicity moderated the relationship between acculturation and unsafe sex.

Regarding the mediating effect, especially sex guilt mediated the relationship between mainstream acculturation and sexual desire (Brotto et al., 2012; Woo et al., 2011, 2012). This is in line with what was provided in the introduction. The acculturation process indirectly influences sexual health.

This systematic review and some reviewed studies are subject to some limitations. In relation to acculturation measures, proxy indicators such as the length of residency may be a weak measure. For instance, years in Canada may not capture the changes that accompany immersion into a new culture (Brotto et al., 2005). Furthermore, the diversity of acculturation measures used makes it difficult to compare the results. Regarding the sexual attitude measures, the diversity of concepts used (sexual permissiveness, sexual conservatism, etc.) also makes it difficult to compare the results. Finally, the sexual attitude measures may not adequately capture the sexual attitudes of minority groups. This could also explain the discrepancy in the relationship between heritage acculturation and sexual attitudes found in some studies. As other research suggests (Dang et al., 2019), future studies should use sexual attitude measures designed specifically for minority groups living in Western countries. Moreover, future studies should deeply examine the relationship between the acculturation process and attitudes toward more specific aspects of sexuality, such as attitudes toward same-sex orientations o behaviors.

Conclusion

This systematic literature review demonstrates that the acculturation process is related to sexual attitudes. While mainstream acculturation is positively related to liberal or positive sexual attitudes, the relationship between heritage acculturation and sexual attitudes does not point in the same direction in all studies. In addition, relevant interaction and mediation effects were also found: (1) mainstream dimension moderates the relationship between heritage dimension and sexual attitudes, (2) heritage dimension moderates the relationship between mainstream dimension and sexual attitudes, (3) acculturation moderates the relation between gender and sexual attitudes, (4) gender moderates the relation between acculturation and sexual attitudes, (5) the ethnocultural origin moderates the relationship between acculturation and sexual attitudes, and (6) sex guilt mediated the relationship between mainstream acculturation and sexual desire.

Social Policy Implications

Because sexual attitudes are related to sexual health (e.g., Carvalho et al., 2013) and sexual activity (e.g., Blanc et al., 2018), knowing the acculturation process could be important to achieve greater equity in sexual health among different groups.

The studies provide strong evidence that the sexual attitudes can best be understood by examining the role of acculturation. The sexual attitudes of minority groups are related to the acculturation strategies used. Therefore, it would be very useful to promote the most suitable acculturation strategy in the development of prevention and intervention programs in sexual health. The results of the reviewed studies are also relevant to sexual educators working with diverse ethnic groups because they provide understanding of the factors related to sexuality in multicultural contexts. Likewise, it could also be useful for clinical sexologists to administer acculturation measures to patients with sexual dysfunctions.