Skip to main content
Log in

The “Non-canonical” Solution and the “Improvisation” as Conditions for Early Years Mathematics Learning Processes: The Concept of the “Interactional Niche in the Development of Mathematical Thinking“ (NMT)

Die „nicht-kanonische“ Lösung und die „Improvisation“ als Bedingungen des Mathematiklernens im Kindergarten- und frühen Grundschulalter: die interaktionale Nische mathematischer Denkentwicklung (NMD)

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal für Mathematik-Didaktik Aims and scope Submit manuscript

“In principle, extra ‘depth’ in some metaphorical sense is to be expected whenever the information for … two descriptions is differently collected or differently coded” (Gregory Bateson 1979, p. 79).

Abstract

By introducing the concept of the “interactional niche in the development of mathematical thinking” (NMT), a new approach, in which the cultural historic and the interactionistic strands of socio-constructivist theories merge, will be presented. This concept is to be understood as an attempt to develop a theory about the generation of mathematical thinking of children in the ages between three and ten. Two specific niches will be described that arise under the condition of children’s non-canonical solutions and corresponding improvisation by (nursery) teacher and children.

Zusammenfassung

Es wird der Begriff der „interaktionalen Nische mathematischer Denkentwicklung“ (NMD) eingeführt. Mit ihm werden der kulturhistorische und der interaktionistische Ansatz im Rahmen sozialkonstruktivistischer Theorie verbunden. Er stellt ein Grundkonzept für eine zu entwickelnde Theorie mathematischer Denkentwicklung für Kinder im Alter von 3 bis 10 Jahren dar. Es werden zwei spezifische Nischen beschrieben, die unter der Bedingung ungewöhnlicher Lösungen von Kindern und darauf bezogener Improvisationen von Erzieherin, Lehrerin und den Kindern rekonstruiert werden konnten.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The empirical background is the project “early Steps in Mathematics Learning” (erStMaL) that is part of the research activities of the “Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education” (IDeA) located in Frankfurt am Main, Germany. It has been funded for 6 years, beginning in July 2008; http://www.idea-frankfurt.eu/homepage/idea-projects/projekt-erstmal.

  2. In the English-speaking world, the word “kindergarten” usually means an institution in which children spend just one year before entering primary school from age 5 to 6. In Germany, the word “Kindergarten” designates the institution, which children visit from ages 3 to 6 before they start primary school. In the US, one might use the abbreviation “pre-k”. In its organization and administration, the German Kindergarten is separate from primary schools. In order to remind the reader that my data originate from a German institution, I consequently write this word with a capital letter, as one usually does for nouns in German.

  3. This scene is first mentioned in Krummheuer (2011a).

  4. The chipmunk, a stuffed animal, is the mascot of the project erStMaL.

  5. In the original German transcript, we do not use the standard interpunctuation, and denote speaking pauses, raisings of the pinch, and so forth. In the English translation, we do not use these paralinguistic notations. Word order and tone of voice differ too much between German and English. In the Appendix, I attach the original German transcript. The numbering of the transcript lines is identical.

  6. For the notion “didactical design pattern”, see for example Acar Bayraktar et al. (Acar Bayraktar et al. 2011), p. 17 f. and below Sect. 4.

  7. This scene is first mentioned in Krummheuer (1997).

  8. The translation into English does not transport the double meaning of this second sentence. It also could be translated as “Four friends play together”.

  9. See the concept of “format” (Bruner 1983) and “scaffolding” (Bruner 1996), Vygotsky’s notion of “zone of proximal development” (Wertsch and Tulviste 1992), or the approach of “legitimate peripheral participation” (Lave and Wenger 1991).

  10. Surprisingly, Ernest (2010) does not mention this research tradition that usually is subsumed under the name “micro-sociology”. For its reception in mathematics education, see Bauersfeld (1995), Krummheuer (1995), and Voigt (1995).

  11. Leeway taken here in the colloquial meaning of “room for freedom of action” (see Webster 1983, p. 1034), originally the notion of “Partizipationsspielraum” (Brandt 2004).

  12. In the wording of Prediger et al. (2008), I could speak of a “networking strategy”: … “networking strategies are those connecting strategies that respect on the one hand the pluralism and/or modularity of autonomous theoretical approaches but are on the other hand concerned with reducing the unconnected multiplicity of theoretical approaches…” (p. 170).

  13. Culture taken here either as a macro-sociological global precondition or as a micro-sociological phenomenon of locally stabilized and routinized procedures of meaning negotiation.

  14. The table is published in Krummheuer Krummheuer 2011a, p. 65.

  15. Thanks to one of the reviewers for this alternative interpretation.

  16. The notion of “overhearer“ belongs to four concepts of the “recipient design” as outlined in Krummheuer (2011b). It stands for the status of a recipient that is not directly addressed by the priorly speaking or acting person but is tolerated as listener and observer. In the given scene, the children always address their utterances and actions to the adult person B and define by this the status of the remaining children as that of overhearers.

  17. In Sect. 2, I mentioned Prediger et al. (2008) and their concept of “networking strategies”. My aim, of course, is to synthesize these different aspects within the proposed conceptual framework on an NMT. In words of Cobb (2007), one can speak of an attempt of “theorizing as bricolage” (p. 16) • a metaphorization that one can already find in Lévi Strauss (1966); see also Lawler (1980).

References

  • Acar Bayraktar, E., Hümmer, A.-M., Huth, M., Münz, M., & Reimann, M. (2011). Forschungsmethodischer Rahmen der projekte erStMaL und MaKreKi. In B. Brandt, R. Vogel, & G. Krummheuer (Eds.), Mathematikdidaktische Forschung am „Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education“. Grundlagen und erste Ergebnisse der Projekte erStMaL und MaKreKi. Münster: Waxmann (Vol. 1, pp. 11–24).

    Google Scholar 

  • Acar Bayraktar, E., & Krummheuer, G. (2011). Die Thematisierung von Lagebeziehungen und Perspektiven in zwei familialen Spielsituationen. Erste Einsichten in die Struktur „interaktionaler Nischen mathematischer Denkentwicklung“ im familialen Kontext. In B. Brandt, R. Vogel, & G. Krummheuer (Eds.), Mathematikdidaktische Forschung am „Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education“. Grundlagen und erste Ergebnisse der Projekte erStMaL und MaKreKi, Münster: Waxmann (Vol. 1, pp. 135–174).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and nature. Toronto: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauersfeld, H. (1980). Hidden dimensions in the so-called reality of mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(1), 23–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauersfeld, H. (1995). “Language games” in the mathematics classroom: their function and their effects. In P. Cobb & H. Bauersfeld (Eds.), The emergence of mathematical meaning. Interaction in classroom cultures (pp. 271–291). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandt, B. (2004). Kinder als Lernende. Partizipationsspielräume und -profile im Klassenzimmer. Frankfurt a. M.: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk. Learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (2007). Putting philosophy to work: coping with multiple theoretical perspectives. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 3–38). Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, F. (1982). Classroom discourse as improvisation. In L. C. Wilkinson (Ed.), Communicating in the classroom (pp. 153–181). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernest, P. (2010). Reflections on theories of learning. In B. Sriraman & L. English (Eds.), Theories of mathematics education: seeking new frontiers (pp. 39–46). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1972). Remarks on ethnomethodology. In J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics: the ethnography of communication (pp. 71–96). New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gellert, U. (2008). Validity and relevance: comparing and combining two sociological perspectives on mathematics classroom practice. ZDM. Zentralblatt für Didaktik der Mathematik, 40(2), 215–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1983). The interaction order. American Sociological Review, 48, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harkness, S., Super, C. M., et al. (2007). Culture and the construction of habits in daily life: implications for the successful development of children with disabilities. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 27(4), 33S–30S (Fall Suppl.).

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (1995). The ethnography of argumentation. In P. Cobb & H. Bauersfeld (Eds.), The emergence of mathematical meaning. Interaction in classroom cultures (pp. 229–269). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (1997). Narrativität und lernen. Mikrosoziologische Studien zur sozialen Konstitution schulischen Lernens. Weinheim: Deutscher Studien Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (2007). Argumentation and participation in the primary mathematics classroom. Two episodes and related theoretical abductions. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 26(1), 60–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (2011a). Die empirisch begründete herleitung des begriffs der „interaktionalen nische mathematischer Denkentwicklung“ (NMD). In N. M. D. In, B. Brandt, R. Vogel, & G. Krummheuer (Eds.), Mathematikdidaktische Forschung am „Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education“. Grundlagen und erste Ergebnisse der Projekte erStMaL und MaKreKi, Münster: Waxmann (Vol. 1, pp. 25–89).

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (2011b). Representation of the notion “learning-as-participation” in everyday situations of mathematics classes. ZDM. Zentralblatt für Didaktik der Mathematik, 43(1/2), 81–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, W., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning. Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler, W. (1980). The progressive construction of mind. Cognitive Science, 5, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lévi Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pimm, D. (1987). Speaking mathematically: communication in mathematics classrooms. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prediger, S., Bikner-Ahsbahs, A., et al. (2008). Networking strategies and methods for connecting theoretical approaches: first steps towards a conceptual framework. ZDM. Zentralblatt für Didaktik der Mathematik, 40, 165–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schütte, M. (2009). Sprache und Interaktion im Mathematikunterricht der Grundschule. Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schütz, A., & Luckmann, T. (1979). In Strukturen der Lebenswelt. Frankfurt a. M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: a conceputalization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 9, 545–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobias, S. & Duffy, T. M. (Eds.) (2009). Constructivist instruction. Success of failure? New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, R., & Huth, M. (2010). …und der Elephant in die Mitte – Rekonstruktion mathematischer Konzepte von Kindern in Gesprächssituationen. In B. Brandt, M. Fetzer, & M. Schütte (Eds.), Auf den Spuren Interpretativer Unterrichtsforschung in der Mathematikdidaktik. Götz Krummheuer zum 60. Geburtstag (pp. 177–207). Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voigt, J. (1995). Thematic patterns of interaction and sociomathematical norms. The emergemce of mathematical meaning: interaction in classroom cultures. In P. Cobb & H. Bauersfeld (Eds.), The emergence of mathematical meaning. Interaction in classroom cultures (pp. 163–201). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster, N. (1983). Webster’s new twentieth century dictionary Unabriged. McKechnie, J.L. (2nd ed.). New York: Simon & Shuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V., & Tulviste, P. (1992). L. S. Vygotsky and contemporary developmental psychologiy. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 548–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Götz Krummheuer.

Appendix: Tables 5, 6, 7

Appendix: Tables 5, 6, 7

Table 5 The original transcript of the birthday party scene
Table 6 The original transcript of the marble scene
Table 7 Rules of transcription

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Krummheuer, G. The “Non-canonical” Solution and the “Improvisation” as Conditions for Early Years Mathematics Learning Processes: The Concept of the “Interactional Niche in the Development of Mathematical Thinking“ (NMT). J Math Didakt 33, 317–338 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13138-012-0040-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13138-012-0040-z

Keywords

Mathematics Subject Classification (2010)

Navigation