Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Mindfulness Meditation on Self-Transcendent States: Perceived Body Boundaries and Spatial Frames of Reference

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Mindfulness training is believed to encourage self-transcendent states, but little research has examined this hypothesis. This study examined the effects of mindfulness training on two phenomenological features of self-transcendence: (1) perceived body boundary dissolution and (2) allocentric spatial frame of reference.

Methods

A sample of healthy, young adults (n = 45) were randomized to five sessions of mindfulness training or an active listening control condition.

Results

Results indicated that mindfulness training decreased perceived body boundaries (F4,172 = 6.010, p < .001, η2 = .12) and encouraged more allocentric frames of reference (F4,168 = 2.586, p = .039, η2 = .06). The expected inverse relationship was observed between perceived body boundaries and allocentric frames of reference ((β = − .58, p = .001)), and path analysis revealed that the effect of mindfulness training on allocentric frames of reference was mediated by decreased perceived body boundaries (β = .24, se = .17, CI: 0.11 to 0.78).

Conclusions

Taken together, study results suggest that mindfulness training alters practitioners’ experience of self, relaxing the boundaries of the self and extending the spatial frame of reference further beyond the physical body. Future studies are needed to explore the psychophysiological changes that co-occur with phenomenological reports of self-transcendence and the behavioral consequences following self-transcendent experiences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alsmith, A. J., & Longo, M. R. (2014). Where exactly am I? Self-location judgements distribute between head and torso. Consciousness and Cognition, 24, 70–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anglin, S. M. (2014). I think, therefore I am? Examining conceptions of the self, soul, and mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 105–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 596-612.

  • Ataria, Y., Dor-Ziderman, Y., & Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2015). How does it feel to lack a sense of boundaries? A case study of a long-term mindfulness meditator. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 133–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. H. (2000). Consciousness evolves when the self dissolves. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(11–12), 209–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. H. (2006). Zen-brain reflections: reviewing recent development in meditation and states of consciousness. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. H. (2009). Selfless insight: Zen and the meditative transformations of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., & Glicksohn, J. (2014). The consciousness state space (CSS)—a unifying model for consciousness and self. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 341.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., & Glicksohn, J. (2017). Meditation, absorption, transcendent experience, and affect: tying it all together via the consciousness state space (CSS) model. Mindfulness, 8(1), 68-77.

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2012). Mindfulness-induced changes in gamma band activity–implications for the default mode network, self-reference and attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(4), 700–710.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, A. E., & Stevens, L. (2015). EEG manifestations of nondual experiences in meditators. Consciousness and Cognition, 31, 1–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertossa, F., Besa, M., Ferrari, R., & Ferri, F. (2008). Point zero: a phenomenological inquiry into the seat of consciousness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107(2), 323–335.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y.-Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 20254–20259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: a review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 15, 593–600.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., Cosmelli, D., Legrand, D., & Thompson, E. (2011). Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 104–112.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, C. J., Lutz, A., & Davidson, R. J. (2015). Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(9), 515–523.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dambrun, M. (2016). When the dissolution of perceived body boundaries elicits happiness: the effect of selflessness induced by a body scan meditation. Consciousness and Cognition, 46, 89–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dambrun, M., & Ricard, M. (2011). Self-centeredness and selflessness: a theory of self-based psychological functioning and its consequences for happiness. Review of General Psychology, 15(2), 138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demarzo, M. M. P., Montero-Marin, J., Cuijpers, P., Zabaleta-del-Olmo, E., Mahtani, K. R., Vellinga, A., Vicens, C., López-del-Hoyo, Y., & García-Campayo, J. (2015). The efficacy of mindfulness-based interventions in primary care: a meta-analytic review. Annals of Family Medicine, 13(6), 573–582.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Depraz, N., Varela, F., & Vermersch, P. (2003). On becoming aware. Philadelphia: John Benjamins North America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013). Mindfulness-induced selflessness: a MEG neurophenomenological study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.

  • Dreyfus, G., & Thompson, E. (2007). Asian perspectives: Indian theories of mind.  In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 89-114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,

  • Dreyfus, G. (2011). Is mindfulness present-centred and non-judgmental? A discussion of the cognitive dimensions of mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(01), 41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Froeliger, B., Garland, E. L., Kozink, R. V., Modlin, L. A., Chen, N.-K., McClernon, F. J., Greeson, J. M., & Sobin, P. (2012). Meditation-state functional connectivity (msFC): strengthening of the dorsal attention network and beyond. Evidence-based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 2012, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 14–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2013). A pattern theory of self. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 443.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Garland, E. L. (2013). Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for addiction, stress, and pain. Washington, DC: NASW Press.

  • Goldberg, S. B., Tucker, R. P., Greene, P. A., Davidson, R. J., Wampold, B. E., Kearney, D. J., & Simpson, T. L. (2018). Mindfulness-based interventions for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 59, 52–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gotink, R. A., Chu, P., Busschbach, J. J., Benson, H., Fricchione, G. L., & Hunink, M. M. (2015). Standardised mindfulness-based interventions in healthcare: an overview of systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs. PLoS One, 10(4), e0124344.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, P., & Van Dam, N. T. (2011). Mindfulness, by any other name…: trials and tribulations of sati in western psychology and science. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(01), 219–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gyamtso, K. (1994). Progressive stages of meditation on emptiness. Oxford: Longchen Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, A. W., Nakamura, Y., & Garland, E. L. (2018). The nondual awareness dimensional assessment (NADA): new tools to assess nondual traits and states of consciousness occurring within and beyond the context of meditation. Psychological Assessment, 30(12), 1625-1639.

  • Hanley, A. W., & Garland, E. L. (2019). Spatial frame of reference as a phenomenological feature of self-transcendence: Measurement and manipulation through mindfulness meditation. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(4), 329–345.

  • Hood Jr., R. W. (1975). The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 14, 29–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, J., Napper, E., & Lama, H. D. (1984). Kindness, clarity, and insight: the fourteenth Dalai Lama. Ithaca, NY: Snow Lion Publications.

  • Igelström, K. M., & Graziano, M. S. (2017). The inferior parietal lobule and temporoparietal junction: a network perspective. Neuropsychologia, 105, 70–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co..

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khoury, B., Sharma, M., Rush, S. E., & Fournier, C. (2015). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals: a meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 78(6), 519–528.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klatzky, R. L. (1998). Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, and K. F. Wender (eds), Spatial Cognition: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Representation and Processing of SpatialKnowledge (pp. 1–17). Berlin: Springer.

  • Legrand, D., & Ruby, P. (2009). What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychological Review, 116(1), 252.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Limanowski, J., & Hecht, H. (2011). Where do we stand on locating the self? Psychology, 2(04), 312-317.

  • Lin, Y., Callahan, C. P., & Moser, J. S. (2018). A mind full of self: Self-referential processing as a mechanism underlying the therapeutic effects of mindfulness training on internalizing disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 172–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macy, J. (1991). Mutual causality in Buddhism and general systems theory: the dharma of natural systems. Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macy, J. R. (1979). Dependent co-arising: the distinctiveness of Buddhist ethics. The Journal of Religious Ethics, 7, 38–52.

  • Medina, J., Kannan, V., Pawlak, M. A., Kleinman, J. T., Newhart, M., Davis, C., Heidler-Gary, J. E., Herskovits, E. H., & Hillis, A. E. (2009). Neural substrates of visuospatial processing in distinct reference frames: evidence from unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2073–2084.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Millière, R., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Roseman, L., Trautwein, F.-M., & Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2018). Psychedelics, meditation, and self-consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 9.

  • Rahula, W. (2007). What the Buddha taught. New York: Grove Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starmans, C., & Bloom, P. (2012). Windows to the soul: children and adults see the eyes as the location of the self. Cognition, 123(2), 313–318.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, G. (1999). The self and the SESMET. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 99–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y.-Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 213–225.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.

  • Volcic, R., & Kappers, A. M. (2008). Allocentric and egocentric reference frames in the processing of three-dimensional haptic space. Experimental Brain Research, 188(2), 199–213.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wahbeh, H., Sagher, A., Back, W., Pundhir, P., & Travis, F. (2018). A systematic review of transcendent states across meditation and contemplative traditions. EXPLORE, 14(1), 19–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, B. A., & Hodel, B. (2008). Embracing mind: the common ground of science and spirituality. Boulder: Shambhala Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, A. (1957). The way of Zen. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood Jr., R. W., Vago, D. R., & Newberg, A. B. (2017). The varieties of self-transcendent experience. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 143–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010a). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 597–605.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Gordon, N. S., & Goolkasian, P. (2010b). Effects of brief and sham mindfulness meditation on mood and cardiovascular variables. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(8), 867–873.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Adam Hanley was supported by a Varela Award from the Mind and Life Institute. Eric Garland was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA042033) and the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (R61AT009296).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AWH: reviewed the literature and summarized previous, related work; conducted the analyses; and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MD: collaborated with the design and writing of the final manuscript. ELG: collaborated with the design and writing of the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eric L. Garland.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Adam Hanley, Michael Dambrun, and Eric Garland declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Ethical Approval

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Utah in 2016 (IRB_00088540).

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in this study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hanley, A.W., Dambrun, M. & Garland, E.L. Effects of Mindfulness Meditation on Self-Transcendent States: Perceived Body Boundaries and Spatial Frames of Reference. Mindfulness 11, 1194–1203 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01330-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01330-9

Keywords

Navigation