Skip to main content
Log in

Belief in Race as Biological: Early Life Influences, Intergroup Outcomes, and the Process of “Unlearning”

  • Published:
Race and Social Problems Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper reports on three studies examining people’s belief about race as biological. Study 1 (N = 155) found that the relationship between early exposure to diversity as a child and belief in race as biological was moderated by socioeconomic status. Study 2 (N = 210) found that belief in race as biological was related to greater social distance toward out-group members, and this relationship was mediated by out-group discomfort. Study 3 found that participants (N = 31) had significant decreases in belief in race as biological immediately following a daylong race relations workshop and in a 6-week follow-up, and this “unlearning” trajectory was particularly prominent among students who experienced greater social distance from out-groups. Results are interpreted and discussed in relation to reconceptualizing beliefs about race as rationalizing rather than proactive ideologies and for promoting positive race relations through education aimed at deessentializing race.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A related body of research has established that “psychological essentialism” more generally is also related to negative intergroup outcomes (Bastian and Haslam 2006; Haslam et al. 2002; Jayaratne 2006; Keller 2005; Plaks et al. 2012). Given this focus in this research on beliefs about race specifically, this supporting research is deemed outside the scope of this review.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. New York: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 228–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumer, H. (1958). Race prejudice as a sense of group position. Pacific Sociological Review, 1(1), 3–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogardus, E. S. (1925). Measuring social distances. Journal of Applied Sociology (University of Southern California), 9(4), 299–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowman, N. A., & Denson, N. (2012). What’s past is prologue: How precollege exposure to racial diversity shapes the impact of college interracial interactions. Research in Higher Education, 53, 406–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, M. M., Chen, J., Roisman, G. I., & Hong, Y. (2007). Essentializing race: Implications for bicultural individuals’ cognition and physiological reactivity. Psychological Science, 18(4), 341–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, M. M., Hong, Y-y, & Chiu, C-y. (2013). Essentializing race: Its implications on racial categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 619–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, E. R., & Yip, T. (2008). Using outgroup comfort to predict black college students’ experiences. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14(1), 57–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, B. (2014). Playing with fire? The impact of hidden curriculum in school genetics on essentialist conceptions of race. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 51(4), 462–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaither, S. E., Schultz, J. R., Pauker, K., Sommers, S. R., Maddox, K. B., & Ambady, N. (2013). Essentialist thinking predicts decrements in children’s memory for racially ambiguous faces. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 482–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2002). Are essentialist beliefs associated with prejudice? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 87–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jayaratne, T. (2006). White Americans’ genetic lay theories of race differences and sexual orientation: Their relationship with prejudice towards blacks, and gay men and lesbians. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9(1), 77–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: The biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(4), 686–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, J. (1996). Science and race. American Behavioral Scientists, 40(2), 123–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44(12), 1469–1481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morning, A. (2008). Reconstructing race in science and society: Biology textbooks, 1952–2002. American Journal of Sociology, 114, 106–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, T. A., Hornsey, M. J., & Postmes, T. (2009). Shifting ground: The variable use of essentialism in contexts of inclusion and exclusion. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 35–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • No, S., Hong, Y-y, Liao, H.-Y., Lee, K., Wood, D., & Chao, M. M. (2008). Lay theories of race affects and moderates Asian Americans’ responses towards American culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 991–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Omi, M., & Winant, H. (1994). Racial formation in the United States: From the 1960s to the 1990s. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascarella, E. T., Martin, G. L., Hanson, J. M., Trolian, T. L., Gillig, B., & Blaich, C. (2014). Effects of diversity experiences on critical thinking skills over 4 years of college. Journal of College Student Development, 55(1), 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., Malahy, L. W., Sedlins, M., & Shoda, Y. (2012). Folk beliefs about human genetic variation predict discrete versus continuous racial categorization and evaluative bias. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(1), 31–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quillian, L., & Redd, R. (2009). The friendship networks of multiracial adolescents. Social Science Research, 38, 279–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rollock, D., & Vrana, S. R. (2005). Ethnic social comfort I: Construct validity through social distance measurement. Journal of Black Psychology, 31(4), 386–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, K., Hewstone, M., & Al Ramiah, A. (2012). Neighborhood diversity and social identity complexity: Implications for intergroup relations. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(2), 135–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. D., & Willet, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis. New York, NY: Oxford Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedley, A., & Smedley, B. D. (2005). Race as biology is fiction, racism as a social problem is real. American Psychologist, 60(1), 16–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup Anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tadmor, C. T., Chao, M. M., Hong, Y-y, & Polzer, J. (2013). Not just for stereotyping anymore: Racial essentialism reduces domain-general creativity. Psychological Science, 24(1), 99–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tadmor, C. T., Hong, Y-y, Chao, M. M., & Wiruchnipawan, F. (2012). Multicultural experiences reduce intergroup bias through epistemic unfreezing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 750–772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tauriac, J. J., Kim, G. S., Lambe Sariñana, S., Tawa, J. & Kahn, V. D. (2013). Utilizing affinity groups to enhance intergroup dialogue workshops for racially and ethnically diverse students. The Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 38(3), 241–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tawa, J. & Kim, G. S. (2011). How does biological belief in race relate to our feelings towards in-group and out-groups? A cognitive dissonance framework. Current Research in Social Psychology, 16(4).

  • White, K. R. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, M. J., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2008). Biological conceptions of race and the motivation to cross racial boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 1033–1047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, Y. J., Horn, A. J., & Chen, S. (2013). Perceived masculinity: The potential influence of race, racial essentialist beliefs, and stereotypes. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 14(4), 452–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank his undergraduate and graduate mentor, Dr. Karen Suyemoto, with whom he shares ownership for datasets in Study 1 and Study 2. The author would also like to thank his colleagues with whom the intervention and data collection in Study 3 were completed with: Dr. Jesse Tauriac, Dr. Grace Kim, Dr. Susan Lambe Sariñana, and Dr. Vali Kahn.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Tawa.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tawa, J. Belief in Race as Biological: Early Life Influences, Intergroup Outcomes, and the Process of “Unlearning”. Race Soc Probl 8, 244–255 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12552-016-9176-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12552-016-9176-7

Keywords

Navigation