Skip to main content
Log in

Teaching Robots a Lesson: Determinants of Robot Punishment

  • Published:
International Journal of Social Robotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There have been multiple incidents where humans attacked robots in a public environment (Brscić et al., in: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, ACM/IEEE, Portland, 2015, https://doi.org/10.1145/2696454.2696468); Vincent, in: A drunk man was arrested for knocking over Silicon Valley’s crime-fighting robot, 2017, https://www.theverge.com/2017/4/26/15432280/security-robot-knocked-over-drunk-man-knightscope-k5-mountain-view; Mosbergen, in: Good job, America. You killed hitchBOT. Huffpost, 2015, https://www.huffpost.com/entry/hitchbot-destroyed-philadelphia_n_55bf24cde4b0b23e3ce32a67; Mutlu and Forlizzi, in: Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on Human robot interaction, ACM, 2008, https://doi.org/10.1145/2696454.2696468; Rehm and Krogsager, in: 2013 Proceedings of IEEE RO-MAN, IEEE, 2013, https://doi.org/10.1145/2696454.2696468; (Salvini et al., in: 19th International symposium in robot and human interactive communication, 2010). Although the form of aggression suggests that this behaviour might be motivated by the aggressor’s desire for social recognition rather than an urge for vandalism (Salvini et al. 2010; Keijsers and Bartneck, in: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, ACM/IEEE, New York, 2018, https://doi.org/10.1145/2696454.2696468), very little is known about the underlying psychological mechanisms. Therefore, extending previous research, the current study investigated if human aggression towards a robot would be influenced by the aggressor’s feelings of power, the perception of the threat that robots in general might pose, mind attribution to the robot, and the robot’s embodiment. First, threat and power were manipulated. Subsequently, participants played a learning task with either a virtual or an embodied robot. Mind attribution was measured afterwards. Participants were asked to restrict the robot’s energy supply after each wrong answer, which was taken as a measure of aggression. Results indicated that an embodied robot was punished less harshly than a virtual one, except for when people had been primed with power and threat. Being primed with power diminished the influence of mind attribution. Mind attribution increased aggression in the threat condition but was related to decreased aggression when people had not been reminded of threat.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Threat condition video: https://youtu.be/GquL-MofDbg.

  2. Control condition video: https://youtu.be/8rdV4Ah8TI8.

References

  1. Adobe Systems Software (2017) Adobe After Effects CC for MacOS (14.2.1) [Computer software]

  2. Aldebaran Robotics, SoftBank Group (2014) Choregraphe for MacOS (2.1.4) [Computer software]

  3. Apple Inc (1995-2016) TextEdit (Version 1.12 (329)) [Computer software], voice “Junior”

  4. Bain P, Park J, Kwok C, Haslam N (2009) Attributing human uniqueness and human nature to cultural groups: distinct forms of subtle dehumanization. Group Process Intergroup Relat 12(6):789–805. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430209340415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bartneck C, Hu J (2008) Exploring the abuse of robots. Interact Stud 9(3):415–433. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.9.3.04bar

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bartneck C, Rosalia C, Menges R, Deckers I (2005) Robot abuse—a limitation of the media equation. In: Proceedings of the Interact 2005 Workshop on agent abuse, designed intelligence, Rome, Italy. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/4FXQ6

  7. Bartneck C, Van Der Hoek M, Mubin O, Al Mahmud A (2007) Daisy, Daisy, give me your answer do! Switching off a robot. In: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, ACM/IEEE, Arlington, USA, pp 217–222. https://doi.org/10.1145/1228716.1228746

  8. Bartneck C, Reichenbach J, Carpenter J (2008) The carrot and the stick—the role of praise and punishment in human–robot interaction. Interact Stud Soc Behav Commun Biol Artif Syst 9(2):179–203. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.9.2.03bar

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Bartneck C, Duenser A, Moltchanova E, Zawieska K (2015) Comparing the similarity of responses received from studies in Amazon’s Mechanical Turk to studies conducted online and with direct recruitment. PlOS One 10(4):e0121595. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121595

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Brahnam S, De Angeli A (2008) Special issue on the abuse and misuse of social agents. Interact Comput 20:287–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intcom.2008.02.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Briggs G, Scheutz M (2014) How robots can affect human behavior: investigating the effects of robotic displays of protest and distress. Int J Soc Robot 6(3):343–355. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34103-8_24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Brscić D, Kidokoro H, Suehiro Y, Kanda T (2015) Escaping from children’s abuse of social robots. In: Proceedings of the International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, ACM/IEEE, Portland, USA, pp 59–66. https://doi.org/10.1145/2696454.2696468

  13. Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD (2011) Amazon’s mechanical turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci 6(1):3–5. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Cañamero LD (2002) Playing the emotion game with Feelix. In: Socially intelligent agents, Springer, pp 69–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-47373-9_8

  15. Croizet JC, Claire T (1998) Extending the concept of stereotype threat to social class: the intellectual underperformance of students from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 24(6):588–594. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298246003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Cronbach LJ (1951) Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16(3):297–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02310555

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  17. Darling K (2012) Extending legal rights to social robots. In: We Robot Conference, University of Miami, University of Miami, Miami, USA, pp 1–24. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2044797

  18. De Angeli A (2006) On verbal abuse towards chatterbots. In: Proceedings of CHI 2006 Workshop on Misuse and Abuse of Interactive Technologies. Montreal, Canada. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intcom.2008.02.004

  19. De Angeli A, Brahnam S (2008) I hate you! Disinhibition with virtual partners. Interact Comput 20(3):302–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intcom.2008.02.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Ensari N, Miller N (2002) The out-group must not be so bad after all. The effects of disclosure, typicality, and salience on intergroup bias. J Personal Soc Psychol 83(2):313. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Eyssel F (2017) An experimental psychological perspective on social robotics. Robot Auton Syst 87:363–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.robot.2016.08.029

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Field A (2009) Discovering statistics using SPSS, 3rd edn. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  23. Fischer P, Greitemeyer T, Kastenmüller A, Frey D, Oßwald S (2007) Terror salience and punishment. Does terror salience induce threat to social order? J Exp Soc Psychol 43(6):964–971

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Galinsky AD, Gruenfeld DH, Magee JC (2003) From power to action. J Personal Soc Psychol 85(3):453–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Galinsky AD, Magee JC, Inesi ME, Gruenfeld DH (2006) Power and perspectives not taken. Psychol Sci 17(12):1068–1074. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01824.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Gazzola V, Rizzolatti G, Wicker B, Keysers C (2007) The anthropomorphic brain: the mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage 35(4):1674–1684. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Gockley R, Bruce A, Forlizzi J, Michalowski M, Mundell A, Rosenthal S, Sellner B, Simmons R, Snipes K, Schultz AC, et al. (2005) Designing robots for long-term social interaction. In: International conference on intelligent robots and systems, IEEE/RSJ, New York, USA, pp 1338–1343. https://doi.org/10.1109/IROS.2005.1545303

  28. Goodwin SA, Gubin A, Fiske ST, Yzerbyt VY (2000) Power can bias impression processes. Stereotyping subordinates by default and by design. Group Process Intergroup Relat 3(3):227–256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Gwinn JD, Judd CM, Park B (2013) Less power= less human? Effects of power differentials on dehumanization. J Exp Soc Psychol 49(3):464–470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.01.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Haslam N (2006) Dehumanization. an integrative review. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 10(3):252–264. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Haslam N, Loughnan S (2014) Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Ann Rev Psychol 65:399–423. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115045

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Haslam N, Loughnan S, Kashima Y, Bain P (2008) Attributing and denying humanness to others. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 19(1):55–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463280801981645

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Hayashi K, Sakamoto D, Kanda T, Shiomi M, Koizumi S, Ishiguro H, Ogasawara T, Hagita N (2007) Humanoid robots as a passive-social medium-a field experiment at a train station. In: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, New York, USA, pp 137–144, https://doi.org/10.1145/1228716.1228735

  34. Keijsers M, Bartneck C (2018) Mindless robots get bullied. In: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, ACM/IEEE, New York, USA, pp 205–214. https://doi.org/10.1145/3171221.3171266

  35. Kozak MN, Marsh AA, Wegner DM (2006) What do I think you’re doing? Action identification and mind attribution. J Personal Soc Psychol 90(4):543–555. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.543

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Krach S, Hegel F, Wrede B, Sagerer G, Binkofski F, Kircher T (2008) Can machines think? Interaction and perspective taking with robots investigated via fMRI. PlOS One 3(7):e2597. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002597

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Kteily N, Bruneau E, Waytz A, Cotterill S (2015) The ascent of man: theoretical and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. J Personal Soc Psychol 109(5):901. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000048

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Lammers J, Stapel DA (2011) Power increases dehumanization. Group Process Intergroup Relat 14(1):113–126. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430210370042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Lapidot-Lefler N, Barak A (2012) Effects of anonymity, invisibility, and lack of eye-contact on toxic online disinhibition. Comput Hum Behav 28(2):434–443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.10.014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Lasica JD (2014) Knightscope K5 at the Launch Festival, held Feb. 24-26, 2014 at San Francisco’s Design Concourse. https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Knightscope_K5_(12809731473).jpg. (Online; recovered 25 June 2019)

  41. Leidner B, Castano E, Ginges J (2013) Dehumanization, retributive and restorative justice, and aggressive versus diplomatic intergroup conflict resolution strategies. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 39(2):181–192. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212472208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Li J (2015) The benefit of being physically present: a survey of experimental works comparing copresent robots, telepresent robots and virtual agents. Int J Hum Comput Stud 77:23–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2015.01.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Locke KD (2009) Aggression, narcissism, self-esteem, and the attribution of desirable and humanizing traits to self versus others. J Res Personal 43(1):99–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.10.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Lowry PB, Zhang J, Wang C, Siponen M (2016) Why do adults engage in cyberbullying on social media? An integration of online disinhibition and deindividuation effects with the social structure and social learning model. Inf Syst Res 27(4):962–986. https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2016.0671

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Modecki KL, Minchin J, Harbaugh AG, Guerra NG, Runions KC (2014) Bullying prevalence across contexts: a meta-analysis measuring cyber and traditional bullying. J Adolesc Health 55(5):602–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.06.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Mosbergen D (2015) Good job, America. You killed hitchBOT. Huffpost https://www.huffpost.com/entry/hitchbot-destroyed-philadelphia_n_55bf24cde4b0b23e3ce32a67

  47. Mutlu B, Forlizzi J (2008) Robots in organizations: The role of workflow, social, and environmental factors in human–robot interaction. In: Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on Human robot interaction, ACM, pp 287–294. https://doi.org/10.1145/1349822.1349860

  48. Nomura T, Kanda T, Kidokoro H, Suehiro Y, Yamada S (2017) Why do children abuse robots? Interact Stud 17(3):347–369. https://doi.org/10.1145/2701973.2701977

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Postigo S, González R, Montoya I, Ordoñez A (2013) Theoretical proposals in bullying research. A review. Anal de Psicol 29(2):413–425

    Google Scholar 

  50. Rosenthal-von der Pütten AM, Krämer NC, Hoffmann L, Sobieraj S, Eimler SC (2013a) An experimental study on emotional reactions towards a robot. Int J Soc Robot 5(1):17–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-012-0173-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Rosenthal-von der Pütten AM, Schulte FP, Eimler SC, Hoffmann L, Sobieraj S, Maderwald S, Krämer NC, Brand M (2013b) Neural correlates of empathy towards robots. In: Proceedings of the 8th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction, IEEE Press, pp 215–216. https://doi.org/10.1109/HRI.2013.6483578

  52. Rehm M, Krogsager A (2013) Negative affect in human robot interaction – impoliteness in unexpected encounters with robots. In: 2013 Proceedings of IEEE RO-MAN, IEEE, pp 45–50. https://doi.org/10.1109/ROMAN.2013.6628529

  53. Rudman LA, Mescher K (2012) Of animals and objects: Men’s implicit dehumanization of women and likelihood of sexual aggression. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 38(6):734–746. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212436401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Salvini P, Ciaravella G, Yu W, Ferri G, Manzi A, Mazzolai B, Laschi C, Oh SR, Dario P (2010) How safe are service robots in urban environments? Bullying a robot. In: 19th international symposium in robot and human interactive communication, RO-MAN, 2010 IEEE, IEEE, Viareggio, Italy, pp 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1109/ROMAN.2010.5654677

  55. Sandoval EB, Brandstetter J, Bartneck C (2016) Can a robot bribe a human? The measurement of the dark side of reciprocity in human robot interaction. In: 11th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction, IEEE, Christchurch, pp 117 – 124. https://doi.org/10.1109/HRI.2016.7451742

  56. Simons DJ, Chabris CF (2012) Common (mis)beliefs about memory: a replication and comparison of telephone and Mechanical Turk survey methods. PlOS One 7(12):e51876. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051876

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Slater M, Antley A, Davison A, Swapp D, Guger C, Barker C, Pistrang N, Sanchez-Vives MV (2006) A virtual reprise of the Stanley Milgram obedience experiments. PlOS One 1(1):e39. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000039

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Suler J (2004) The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychol Behav 7(3):321–326. https://doi.org/10.1089/1094931041291295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Tan XZ, Vázquez M, Carter EJ, Morales CG, Steinfeld A (2018) Inducing bystander interventions during robot abuse with social mechanisms. In: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, ACM/IEEE, New York, USA, pp 169–177. https://doi.org/10.1145/3171221.3171247

  60. Vincent J (2017) A drunk man was arrested for knocking over Silicon Valley’s crime-fighting robot. https://www.theverge.com/2017/4/26/15432280/security-robot-knocked-over-drunk-man-knightscope-k5-mountain-view. (Online; recovered 30 August 2018)

  61. Volk AA, Veenstra R, Espelage DL (2017) So you want to study bullying? Recommendations to enhance the validity, transparency, and compatibility of bullying research. Aggress Viol Behav 36:34–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2017.07.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Waytz A, Epley N (2012) Social connection enables dehumanization. J Exp Soc Psychol 48(1):70–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Whitby B (2008) Sometimes it’s hard to be a robot: a call for action on the ethics of abusing artificial agents. Interact Comput 20(3):326–333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intcom.2008.02.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Woods J (2011) Framing terror: An experimental framing effects study of the perceived threat of terrorism. Crit Stud Terror 4(2):199–217. https://doi.org/10.1080/17539153.2011.586205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Wullenkord R, Fraune MR, Eyssel F, S̆abanović S (2016) Getting in touch: How imagined, actual, and physical contact affect evaluations of robots. In: 2016 25th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (RO-MAN), IEEE, New York, USA, pp 980–985. https://doi.org/10.1109/ROMAN.2016.7745228

  66. Yogeeswaran K, Złotowski J, Livingstone M, Bartneck C, Sumioka H, Ishiguro H (2016) The interactive effects of robot anthropomorphism and robot ability on perceived threat and support for robotics research. J Hum Robot Interact 5(2):29–47. https://doi.org/10.5898/JHRI.5.2.Yogeeswaran

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Young M (2016) What is a robot? https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=S5miA6jXf0E&frags=pl%2Cwn

  68. Zeileis A (2004) Econometric computing with HC and HAC covariance matrix estimators. Research Report Series / Department of Statistics and Mathematics. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v011.i10

  69. Zlotowski J, Yogeeswaran K, Bartneck C (2017) Can we control it? Autonomous robots threaten human identity, uniqueness, safety, and resources. Int J Hum Comput Stud 100:48–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2016.12.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was funded by the University of Canterbury, New Zealand.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Merel Keijsers.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Keijsers, M., Kazmi, H., Eyssel, F. et al. Teaching Robots a Lesson: Determinants of Robot Punishment. Int J of Soc Robotics 13, 41–54 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-019-00608-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-019-00608-w

Keywords

Navigation