Skip to main content
Log in

A Contribution to Understanding Consciousness: Qualia as Phenotype

  • Review
  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this model consciousness is a form of memory. We are essentially “living in the past” as our experience, the qualia, is always of past events. Consciousness represents the storage of past events for use in future situations and it is altered by external experience of the organism. Psychological frameworks of conditioning and learning theory are used to explain this model along with recent neuropsychological research on synaesthesia and phantom limb pain. Consciousness results from the gradual evolutionary development of the human information processing function. Language is hypothesised to have evolved at a pre-conscious stage of human development as a function of the need for ‘within-organism’ data storage. Communication with others may not have been the initial evolutionary advantage conferred by language. The later incidental use of language as a communication tool, which results in the reflecting back of one’s experience through others, is what has triggered a conscious experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armel, K. C., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2003). Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London—Series B, 270(1523), 1499–1506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borges, R. M. (2005). Do plants and animals differ in phenotypic plasticity? Journal of Bioscience, 30(1), 41050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, E., Cristea, A., Stewart, C., & Brailsford, T. (2005). Patterns in authoring of adaptive educational hypermedia: a taxonomy of learning styles. Educational Technology & Society, 8(3), 77–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (1992). Origins of self-perception in Infancy. Psychological Inquiry, 3(2), 103–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. (1995). Towards a theory of consciousness, facing up to the problem of consciousness. MIT Press.

  • Christianson, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2003). Language Evolution, consensus and controversies. Trends in cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 300–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A Framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2).

  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of the species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. Appleton: New York D.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species. The Penguin Press.

  • Descartes, R. (1641). Meditations of first philosophy.

  • Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (2003). Naturalising consciousness, a theoretical framework. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 100(9), 5520–5524.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson, H. H., & Holmes, N. P. (2003). Touching a rubber hand: feeling of body ownership is associated with activity in multisensory brain areas. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, 270(1523), 1499–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., & Chomsky, N. (2005). (2005) The evolution of the language faculty: clarifications and implications. Discussion/Cognition, 97, 179–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grime, J. P., Campbell, B. D., Mackey, J. M. L., & Crick, J. C. (1991). Root plasticity, nitrogen capture and competitive ability. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Plant root growth: an ecological perspective (pp. 381–397). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 29822, 1569–1579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haustein, M. P, Harder, T, Mathis, C., & Wagner, M. (2005) Dewey IDs—the key to find-grained management of XML documents. Proceedings of 20th Brazilian Symposium on Databases, pp. 85–99 (2005).

  • Hayne, H., Boniface, J., & Barr, R. (2000). The development of declarative memory in human infants: age related changes in deferred imitation. Behavioural Neuroscience, 114(1), 77–83.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (reply to Searle, J R). (2005). What is consciousness?. New York Review of Books, 52(11), June 23, 2005,

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E. R. (2001). The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science, 294, 1030–1038.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Karban, R., Baldwin, I. T., Baxer, K. J., Laue, G., & Felton, G. W. (2000). Communication between plants: induced resistance in wild tobacco plants following clipping of neighbouring sagebrush. Oecologia, 125, 66–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez-Garcia, J. F., et al. (2000). Direct targeting of light signals to a promoter element-bound transcription factor. Science, 288, 859 (5 MAY 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a Bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological, activity of the cerebral cortex. New York: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: W. Morrow & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitchford, N.J., & Mullen, K. T. (2001). Conceptualization of perceptual attributes: a special case for color? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 289–314, doi:10.1006/jecp.2001.2634, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (1996). Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London—Series B: Biological Sciences, 263, 377–386.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W. (1998). The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. Brain, 121, 1603–1630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia—A Window Into Perception. Thought and Language Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sasaki, & Tokoro (1999). Evolving learnable neural networks under changing environments with various rates of inheritance of acquired characters: Comparison of Darwinian and Lamarckian Evolution Summer 5, (3), Pages 203–223 © 1999 Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Smith, H. (2000). Phytochromes and light signal perception by plants—an emerging synthesis. Nature, 407, 585–591.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, T., Doi, M., Suetsugu, N., Kagawa, T., Wada, M., & Shumazaki, K.-i. (2001). phot1 and phot2 mediate blue light regulation of stomatal opening. Nature, 414, 656–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velmans, M. (2009). How to define Consciousness and how not to define Consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(5), 139–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge: MIT Press. ISBN 0262731622.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fiona O’Doherty.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

O’Doherty, F. A Contribution to Understanding Consciousness: Qualia as Phenotype. Biosemiotics 6, 191–203 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-012-9140-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-012-9140-x

Keywords

Navigation