Introduction

Allostatic load (AL) is a potentially useful conceptual framework through which to capture physiological dysregulation related to chronic stress [1]. However, relatively few studies have directly assessed the association between subjective experiences of chronic stress in different domains and AL. The current study examined (1) the relationship between chronic stress in health, work, relationship, caregiving, and financial domains with AL scores in a community sample of Mexican-American women and (2) evaluated the contribution of lifestyle factors to the association between chronic stress and AL.

Allostatic Load and its Measurement

AL is an increasingly prominent model in research concerning the physiological and health consequences of chronic stress [2, 3]. The framework asserts that the cumulative burden of stress manifests as physiological dysregulation across multiple, interrelated systems involved in restoring allostasis and maintaining healthy functioning in the presence of internal or environmental demands. This “load” is typically indicated by elevated (or reduced) levels across neuroendocrine markers of the sympathetic–adrenal–medullary (SAM; i.e., epinephrine and norepinephrine) and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) systems (i.e., cortisol) and indicators of cardiovascular and metabolic functioning. SAM and HPA functioning are considered the primary mediators of AL, whereas the latter are viewed as secondary indicators of the primary neuroendocrine pathways. Recent conceptualizations of AL have also incorporated markers of inflammation as primary mediators [47]. AL scores have been shown to predict cardiovascular disease (CVD), all-cause mortality, cognitive dysfunction, and disability [811]. Moreover, there is evidence that AL has a stronger relationship with physical health than its individual components [8, 10] or the metabolic syndrome, which overlaps with secondary manifestations of AL [8]. Although additional research is needed to determine optimal measurement, and to test certain aspects of the model, AL appears to have conceptual utility in capturing the physical toll of chronic stress.

Chronic Stress and Allostatic Load

The AL model postulates that cumulative exposure to stress across the lifespan is a catalyst in physiological dysregulation. Accordingly, stressors that are enduring or frequent should demonstrate marked relationships with AL and with health [12]. Consistent with this view, chronic stress in life domains including work [13], marriage [14, 15], and caregiving [16, 17] predicts health outcomes such as CVD and all-cause mortality. Studies have related these chronic stressors to regulatory function in single physiological systems, but research examining chronic stress and multi-systemic functioning is critically lacking. Indeed, many studies of AL have inferred stress based on social conditions such as low socioeconomic status [5, 18, 19] or low social support [20, 21].

Additional research has shown that individuals reporting greater work stress (e.g., job demands, effort–reward imbalance; 22, 23) evidence higher AL scores. Perceived stress among dementia caregivers has also been related to primary mediators, but not other facets, of AL [24]. Caregivers also evidenced increases in primary mediators from baseline to 1-year follow-up in this study, suggesting an escalation in physiological burden over time. Finally, several reports concerning stress and AL derive from the Social Environment and Biomarkers of Aging Study (SEBAS), which examines a nationally representative sample of older Taiwanese residents. In this cohort, individuals who reported more stressful life events in the preceding 4 years evidenced higher AL scores than those with fewer events [25]. Perceived stress also related to higher AL, with a stronger association in women than in men [26]. Duration of life stress, and several presumed indicators of stress (e.g., low education, widowhood) were unrelated to the primary mediators of AL in the SEBAS cohort. However, women (but not men) reporting more current familial stressors showed higher scores on the primary AL mediators [27].

Notably, the degree of dysregulation associated with chronic stress could depend markedly on subjective distress. For example, studies of caregivers suggest that only those who feel “strained” experience deleterious health outcomes [16]. Stress appraisals and reactions stem from many factors including individual differences, such as personality and coping resources, and the social context in which stress occurs [28]. Thus, seemingly stressful social circumstances, such as low socioeconomic status, or social isolation, might not translate into the same physical, emotional, and behavioral consequences for all individuals. Research that incorporates personal perceptions of stress may provide a more complete picture of how stress relates to AL and health [29].

Allostatic Load in the Latino Population

To date, only limited research has examined AL specifically within Latinos. However, studies of Latinos suggest that discrimination [30, 31] and caregiving stress [32] relate to responses in individual physiological systems. In addition, several recent studies have examined predictors of AL in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) III, a US population-based study including a large, multi-ethnic, representative sample. Analyses have shown that (1) socioeconomic status (SES) predicted AL in Latinos and other ethnic groups [33], (2) neighborhood SES related inversely to AL in Mexican-American and Black but not White participants [19], and (3) poverty related to higher aggregate biological risk among all participants after controlling for race/ethnicity [34]. Additional research in an earlier NHANES cohort revealed that Latinos had higher levels of biological risk than Whites, but lower levels than Blacks [35]. Other studies suggest that US-born Mexican-Americans display greater biological risk than Mexican immigrants [7, 35], and that longer duration of US residence predicts higher AL scores in immigrants [7]. To our knowledge, no prior research has examined the association between chronic stress and AL in Latinos.

The Current Study

The current study expanded on the extant literature in several ways. First, we examined chronic stress in several domains, including personal health problems, health and substance problems in loved ones, work, close relationships, housing, finances, and caregiving, in relation to AL. Second, we considered the implications of stress appraisals, by asking participants not only whether they experienced a chronic stressor, but also how stressful they found the situation to be. Third, we examined these relationships in a well-defined social–cultural group—i.e., middle-aged Mexican-American women with diverse SES, living in a border region of the US—that has been underrepresented in research concerning stress, physiological responses, and health.

We hypothesized that women who were exposed to chronic stress in a given domain would demonstrate elevated levels of AL, particularly if they perceived the exposure to be more subjectively distressing. Prior studies of women provide evidence for effects of caregiver [32, 36], relationship (particularly marital) [3739], work [22, 40], and financial stress [41] in association with physiological dysregulation, and it might be predicted that these stressors would be associated with AL in the current study. However, because it is unclear what the relative predictive utility of these domains might be when examined simultaneously, and other stressors examined in the current study have not been well researched, we did not form hypotheses about the relative impact of the different stressor domains.

Finally, the current study examined lifestyle factors as a possible intervening mechanism connecting chronic stress with AL. Health-damaging behaviors have been associated with stress and affect physiological functioning. However, to date, few studies have explored health behaviors in analyses of AL [7, 34, 42, 43], and most have included them as covariates, rather than explicitly examining their contributions to stress–AL associations. We predicted that women experiencing chronic stress would report more health-damaging behaviors, which would relate directly to AL and contribute to (but not fully explain) the association between chronic stress and AL.

Methods

Participants and Recruitment

The current analyses are based in a study of CVD risk in healthy, middle-aged Mexican-American women living near the (San Diego) California–Mexico border. Participants were randomly recruited via targeted telephone and mail procedures. Women were invited to participate if they were aged 40–65 years, Mexican-American, literate in English or Spanish, and free of major health conditions and medications with autonomic effects. Six hundred and fifty-six women were screened, 365 (55.6%) were eligible, and 323 (88%) participated in some or all portions of the study. The current study includes 301 women who completed the psychosocial and physical exams and had all AL datapoints.

Procedures

Participants were scheduled for two consecutive weekday home visits. During the first visit, a bilingual research assistant obtained informed consent and administered a battery of measures (in Spanish or English) assessing sociodemographic characteristics, behavioral risk factors, health history, and psychosocial variables. Participants were also given materials and instructions for a 12-h, overnight urine collection and fasting. The second visit included a fasting blood draw and physical measurements (blood pressure, height, weight, waist circumference). Participants were instructed to refrain from taking anti-inflammatory medications for at least 48 h, to avoid strenuous exercise and alcohol consumption for at least 24 h, and to avoid consuming caffeine and tobacco for at least 30 min prior to the physical exam. All research staff was thoroughly trained in study procedures, and regular quality control checks were conducted. The San Diego State University and University of California, San Diego institutional review boards approved all study procedures.

Allostatic Load: Parameter Assessment and Operationalization

There is controversy about measurement of AL, including which parameters should be included, how they should be combined, whether they should be represented as continuous or categorical variables, and whether cutoffs should be sample-based or established by clinical criteria (e.g., 44, 45). However, studies comparing distinct measurement approaches have found only modest differences in their predictive utility [4, 8, 29, 46], and a recent analysis supported the existence of an overarching AL factor comprising physiological dysregulation across six sub-parameters and 18 specific neuroendocrine, metabolic, cardiovascular, and inflammatory indicators [6]. The current study included all parameters used in the MacArthur Study of Successful Aging [10, 11], with the exception of dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate, which is less clearly related to health outcomes in women than in men [47, 48]. Thus, we examined neuroendocrine markers of SAM and HPA activity [12-h, overnight urinary norepinephrine, epinephrine, and cortisol excretion], metabolic system functioning [serum high-density lipoprotein cholesterol (HDL-c); the ratio of HDL-c to total serum cholesterol; glycosolated hemoglobin (HbA1c); waist circumference], and cardiovascular system functioning [systolic and diastolic blood pressure (SBP and DBP)]. In addition, consistent with other recent conceptualizations [6, 23, 49, 50], we included markers of inflammation [i.e., high sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP); interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α)], given considerable research suggesting that inflammation is an important component of many disease processes [5153] and physiological adjustment to stress [54]. We adopted the MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging algorithm [10, 11], such that each of the 12 biological parameters was divided into quartiles based on the distribution of scores in the sample, and AL was measured by summing the number of parameters for which the subject fell into the highest risk quartile. The one exception was cortisol, for which individuals with scores in either the upper or lower 12.5% of the distribution were counted as high risk. This approach is consistent with research suggesting that both hypo- and hyper-cortisolism represent states of physiological dysregulation related to psychological stress and health problems [55, 56], and has been used in other AL studies [22, 57]. AL scores ranged from 0 to 12, with higher scores indicating greater dysregulation. Table 1 shows the cut-points used for each AL component.

Table 1 Sample cutoffs for each parameter of allostatic load

SBP and DBP were calculated as the average of the second and third of three readings taken while the participant was in a seated position, with arm elevated to heart level, following 30 min of rest, using an automatic sphygmomanometer shown to be valid and reliable (i.e., Omron HEM 705-CP, Omron Corporation, Kyoto Japan). Waist circumference was calculated as the average of the second and third measurements, assessed by tape measure in centimeters at the narrowest point between the ribs and iliac crest. Urine samples were collected by the participant and refrigerated. Blood for the inflammatory biomarkers and lipids was obtained from a venous draw using either ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid as a preservative or serum. Samples were immediately put on ice and centrifuged and the plasma stored at −70°C until assay.

Inflammatory biomarker and neuroendocrine assays were performed at the University of California San Diego (UCSD) General Clinical Research Center Core Laboratory and the UCSD Clinical Biomarker Laboratory. Circulating levels of CRP, IL-6 and TNF-α were determined in non-freeze thawed samples by commercial enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (MSD, Gaithersburg, Maryland). The intra-assay and inter-assay coefficients of variation are less than 5% and 8%, respectively [58]. Urinary cortisol levels were determined by a commercial radioimmunoassay (Diagnostic Products Corporation, Los Angeles, CA), with intra- and inter-assay CVs of <4% and <7%, respectively. Urinary epinephrine and norepinephrine were measured using a catechol-O-methyltransferase-based radioenzymatic assay that concentrates catecholamines with 81% efficiency and has inter- and intra-assay CVs <10% [59]. Results for norepinephrine, epinephrine, and cortisol were reported in micrograms per gram of creatinine to adjust for body size. Blood specimens used to derive lipids and HbA1c were processed by Quest Diagnostics Laboratories. Serum total cholesterol and HDL-c were measured using enzymatic procedures, following standards set by the Lipid Standardization Program of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [60]. HbA1c was assayed using a Biorad Diomat high-pressure liquid chromatography analyzer.

Chronic Stress Domains

Participants completed a measure asking if they had experienced eight chronic stressors for at least 12 months duration [61]. The response format was on a four-point scale with 0 indicating that the stressor did not occur, 1 indicating occurrence of the stressor but with little or no subjective stress (“not upsetting”), and 2 and 3 indicating occurrence of the stressor with moderate or severe subjective stress (“somewhat upsetting” and “very upsetting”), respectively. The domains queried were: personal health stress, health problems in a family member; drug or alcohol problems in a family member; financial strain; work stress; housing problems; relationship stress; and caregiving stress (i.e., helping a sick, disabled, or frail family member or friend on a regular basis). The measure was translated into Spanish for the current study (forward and back translation with reconciliation by committee).

Lifestyle Factors

Participants reported whether or not they currently smoked and frequency and quantity of alcohol intake (used to estimate alcoholic beverages consumed per month). Dietary behaviors were assessed with two brief screeners that have been shown to be highly correlated with the full length Block Food Frequency Questionnaire in a validation study with a large, multi-ethnic sample [62]. For the current study, we examined total fat (grams) and fiber (grams). Spanish translations of these scales were developed for a prior study of Mexican-Americans with minor modifications to include examples of foods specific to the population [e.g., tortillas, lard; 63]. Exercise levels were assessed with the Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire [64], which asks respondents to state the number of times during a typical week they participated in strenuous or moderate exercise for at least 15 min and provides an estimate of total Metabolic Equivalent of Task Units (i.e., METs) per week. METs provide an estimate of the intensity and energy expenditure of physical activity that is comparable across people of differing body sizes. The measure was translated for the current study as described above. Prior research concerning the English version has demonstrated high 2-month test–retest reliability for vigorous exercise (r = 0.84), acceptable test–retest reliability for moderate exercise (r = 0.36), and adequate criterion-related validity with accelerometer assessments [r = 0.36; 64, 65].

Covariates

Analyses controlled for several factors that could be related to chronic stress, AL, or both, including age, menopausal status, (with those reporting no menstruation for at least 12 months considered post-menopausal), SES (education/income composite, defined below), employment status (employed for wages, or not), marital status (married or living with a partner, or not), and insurance status (any coverage or none). In addition, we controlled for language of survey, a commonly used proxy for acculturation [66], which also accounted for the influence of Spanish versus English versions of the measures. For SES, participants indicated the highest level of education they achieved, from no education to a doctoral or professional degree, recoded into six categories for analysis. Total monthly family income was assessed on an ordinal scale in $500 increments, ranging from less than $500/month to more than $8,000 per month. An education/income composite was created by standardizing and summing the variables. For four participants with missing income data, education only was used to represent SES.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were calculated and all variables were examined for normality. Exercise and alcohol were extremely positively skewed, with a large proportion of women reporting no leisure time physical activity (25.7%) or no alcohol consumption (45.5%). Based on the sample distributions, exercise was categorized into quintile groups, and alcohol was coded into 4 groups, representing those who reported 0–1 drinks, 2–5 drinks, 6–19 drinks, and 20 or more drinks per month.

The primary hypotheses of interest were tested in multiple linear regression analyses. Preliminary diagnostic analyses confirmed that assumptions of ordinary least squares regression were met (results not reported). Two regression models were conducted. In the first model, covariates (age, menopausal status, SES, employment status, marital status, health insurance status, and language of assessment) were entered at step 1, followed by the chronic stress variables at step 2. All chronic stress domains were examined in a single analysis, to determine the association between a given stressor with AL after accounting for the influence of all other stressors. A second model examined if lifestyle factors contributed to the association between chronic stress and AL, with covariates entered at step 1, behavioral factors at step 2, and chronic stressors at step 3. Missing data were excluded on a pairwise basis to maximize power. Given the exploratory nature of the research, and relatively small sample size, we also noted cases where relationships approached significance (i.e., 0.05 < p < 0.10).

Results

As shown in Table 2, the mean age of the sample was approximately 49.8 years (SD = 6.61). The majority completed the questionnaires in Spanish (59.5%), and SES was widely varied. Mean AL score was 3.04 (SD = 2.06). Frequency and severity of chronic stressors varied, with only 10.29% reporting moderate to severe personal health stress and 34.21% reporting moderate to severe financial strain.

Table 2 Sample descriptive statistics (N = 301)

As shown in Table 3, Pearson Product Moment and Point-Biserial correlation coefficients showed that women reporting lower alcohol consumption (p < 0.01) and exercise (p < 0.05) evidenced higher AL scores, whereas smoking and dietary factors did not relate to AL. Women with work stress consumed more alcohol (p < 0.05), and tended to consume more fat (p < 0.10). Women reporting relationship stress showed a trend toward higher alcohol consumption (p < 0.10), and significantly higher fat intake (p < 0.01)

Table 3 Bivariate associations between AL and domains of chronic stress with lifestyle factors

Regressing Allostatic Load on Chronic Stress Domains

Table 4 shows the results of analyses regressing AL scores simultaneously on all chronic stress domains. In model 1, the chronic stressors in aggregate accounted for approximately 7% of the variance in AL scores, after accounting for covariates. Work stress (p < 0.05), financial strain (p < 0.01), and caregiving stress (p < 0.01) were independently associated with higher AL scores. Mean predicted AL scores (controlling for covariates) across responses for these domains are shown in Fig. 1. Women without chronic financial or caregiving stress had mean predicted AL scores that were 1.01 and 0.88 points lower, respectively, than did women with severe stress in those domains. Women with chronic work stress but no subjective distress had mean predicted AL scores 1.15 points lower than those with severe chronic work stress.

Table 4 Results of multiple linear regression analyses regressing allostatic load on chronic stress domains, without (model 1) and with (model 2) control for lifestyle factors
Fig. 1
figure 1

Predicted values for allostatic load, controlling for covariates, across chronic stressor response categories for work stress, financial strain, and caregiving stress

In model 2, lifestyle factors accounted for a significant amount of the variance in AL scores, p < 0.05, after controlling for covariates (Table 4). Higher alcohol consumption related to lower AL scores (p < 0.05). The aggregate influence of chronic stress domains remained statistically significant, and work stress, financial strain, and caregiving stress were again observed to be independent predictors of AL scores (p < 0.05). Interestingly, there was a marginally significant trend for higher relationship stress to be associated with lower AL scores in this analysis. In all cases, the reduction in the weight of the regression coefficients for previously significant chronic stress effects was negligible, indicating that lifestyle factors played a minimal role in explaining the association between chronic stress and AL.

Discussion

The current study examined associations between chronic stress experienced in different domains and AL scores in middle-aged Mexican-American women, and evaluated the contribution of lifestyle factors to these associations. Although prior studies have shown that AL relates to social circumstances presumed to foster stress, only limited research has examined stress perceptions, and no study has, to our knowledge, assessed the association of multiple types of chronic stress with AL in a single study.

In support of the AL framework, we found that greater stress in the domains of work, finances, and caregiving related to higher AL scores. These findings are consistent with prior studies that have shown an association between work stress and AL [22, 23] and between perceived stress and the primary mediators of AL in dementia caregivers [24]. To our knowledge, no prior study has examined the specific relationship of chronic financial stress with AL. Notably, health problems in self or loved ones and relationship stress did not relate significantly to AL in the current sample. These stressors have not previously been studied as individual predictors of AL, although relationship (particularly marital) stress has been associated with specific indicators of physiological dysregulation in prior research (e.g., elevated blood pressure; metabolic dysregulation; 3739).

Given research demonstrating that AL predicts health outcomes including CVD and mortality [811], the current findings suggest that chronic stress, particularly in the areas of work, caregiving, and finances may be relevant to understanding Mexican-American women’s health risks. For example, in a study of older adults [67], each 1 unit increment in a similar measure of AL was associated with more than double the mortality risk after adjusting for age and gender. In the current study, the difference in predicted AL scores (controlling for covariates) between women who experienced severe stress and those who reported no stress was more than 1 unit difference for chronic financial strain and 0.85 unit difference for caregiving stress. A greater than 1 point difference was also observed between women who experienced work stress but did not find it upsetting and those with work stress they found to be severely upsetting. Thus, the findings suggest clinically meaningful increases in health risks associated with severe chronic stress in these domains. However, future research is needed to examine how AL relates to objective health outcomes such as mortality specifically in Latino populations.

It is interesting to note that only certain types of chronic stress were independently associated with AL scores in the current study. This supports the utility of considering and discriminating among multiple domains of stress instead of simply examining overall perceived stress, or counting the number of stressful events. In addition, several factors may help explain why certain stressors did not emerge as AL predictors. First, some participants may have been reluctant to disclose problems in areas that are highly valued (i.e., relationship stress) or that carry social stigma (i.e., family substance abuse), especially given the general trend for Latinos to respond in a socially desirable manner (e.g., relative to non-Latino whites; 68). These tendencies may have led to errors in quantifying exposure to chronic stress. Second, lack of precision in assessment could be a contributing factor. For example, participants might have endorsed “ongoing work stress” because of interpersonal problems with colleagues, low levels of control at work, or failure to advance in their profession. Thus, this domain could conceivably represent either a social or achievement related stressor. As another example, “ongoing problems in a close relationship” might describe conflict with a spouse, child, or friend, and could refer to a relationship with someone seen daily or someone with whom the participant interacts only rarely. Given this range, the physiological consequences of close relationship stress could be expected to vary considerably. Comparatively, items assessing caregiving and financial stress are relatively more straightforward in their interpretations. Moreover, both caregiving and financial stress are likely to have an insidious influence across all aspects of a person’s life. Overall, additional research using more refined assessments of each stress domain is needed to explore the stability of the current findings, and whether they generalize to other social–cultural groups.

A second goal of the current study was to examine the contribution of lifestyle factors to associations between chronic stress and AL [43]. In a prior study of middle-aged Hawaiian residents [69], health behaviors (specifically, less alcohol use, more smoking, and less physical activity) were related to physiological dysregulation in multivariate analyses for men but not women. In SEBAS, greater physiological dysregulation was observed in alcohol abstainers versus non-abstainers, whereas smoking did not relate to AL [43]. Analyses performed in the NHANES III cohort found that heavy drinking was unassociated with AL; whereas smokers had higher AL scores [34]. Lifestyle factors explained a significant amount of variance in AL scores after controlling for covariates in the current study. However, only alcohol consumption emerged as a significant predictor, with greater alcohol consumption predicting lesser AL. It is notable that alcohol consumption was quite moderate in our sample, with only 8.5% of women consuming six or more drinks per month. Prior research indicates that the association between alcohol and health is J-shaped, so that low to moderate levels of consumption are salubrious, whereas heavier drinking predicts increased health risks [e.g., higher cardiovascular and all-cause mortality rates; 70, 71]. The mechanisms explaining these effects are believed to include enhanced HDL-c and insulin sensitivity, and reduced inflammation [72], all of which are reflected in our AL construct. Notably, chronic stressors did not relate consistently to lifestyle factors in our study and consequently, these variables had a negligible role in connecting chronic stress with AL. This suggests a more salient direct relationship between chronic stress and AL. Measurement limitations (e.g., self-report assessments of activity and diet; lack of validation information on Spanish versions) and the fact that few women in our study evidenced high-risk lifestyles may also be relevant to understanding these findings.

The current study adds to the literature by comparing multiple domains of chronic stress in relation to AL, considering subjective stress appraisals, and focusing on an understudied, well-defined sociocultural group. However, the study also must be interpreted in light of its limitations. The cross-sectional nature of the design prohibits conclusions regarding directionality of the observed associations. Although conceptually unlikely, AL might influence chronic stress perceptions. A longitudinal study involving multiple assessments of chronic stress domains and AL over time would provide stronger evidence for the hypothesized relationships. In addition, to minimize participant burden, comprehensive assessments of each chronic stress domain were not administered. The use of a single item to assess each stressor could create interpretive differences that might have diminished our ability to identify chronic stress effects in certain domains. Finally, although there are advantages to focusing on a specific demographic group, given that the impact or relevance of different types of stress may be influenced by social–cultural context among other factors, this approach also limits generalizability. We focused on a relatively small sample of a specific segment of US female Latino residents, and the findings cannot be assumed to generalize beyond this sociodemographic group. Likewise, given that we excluded women with health problems, and endeavored to represent the full range of SES (consistent with target communities’ SES distribution according to census data), it is possible that levels of chronic stress or AL were different (e.g., lower) in the current sample relative to what might have been observed in other samples of Mexican-American women. Indeed, certain stress categories were endorsed infrequently, and this may have attenuated statistical power. Future research addressing each of these limitations is recommended to further explore how specific domains of chronic stress relate to AL in diverse populations.