Skip to main content
Log in

Clinical Implications of Numeracy: Theory and Practice

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Annals of Behavioral Medicine

Abstract

Background

Low numeracy is pervasive and constrains informed patient choice, reduces medication compliance, limits access to treatments, impairs risk communication, and affects medical outcomes; therefore, it is incumbent upon providers to minimize its adverse effects.

Purpose

We provide an overview of research on health numeracy and discuss its implications in clinical contexts.

Conclusions

Low numeracy cannot be reliably inferred on the basis of patients’ education, intelligence, or other observable characteristics. Objective and subjective assessments of numeracy are available in short forms and could be used to tailor health communication. Low scorers on these assessments are subject to cognitive biases, irrelevant cues (e.g., mood), and sharper temporal discounting. Because prevention of the leading causes of death (e.g., cancer and cardiovascular disease) depends on taking action now to prevent serious consequences later, those low in numeracy are likely to require more explanation of risk to engage in prevention behaviors. Visual displays can be used to make numerical relations more transparent, and different types of displays have different effects (e.g., greater risk avoidance). Ironically, superior quantitative processing seems to be achieved by focusing on qualitative gist and affective meaning, which has important implications for empowering patients to take advantage of the evidence in evidence-based medicine.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS 2005). National Cancer Institute. Available at: http://hints.matthewsgroup.com/register/register.aspx. Accessibility verified December 10, 2007.

  2. Kirsch I, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. Adult literacy in America: A First Look at the Findings of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, US Department of Education; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Kutner M, Greenberg E, Jin Y, Paulsen C. The Health Literacy of Americas Adults: Results from the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy (NCES 2006-483). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, US Department of Education; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  4. US Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2010. With Understanding and Improving Health and Objectives for Improving Health. 2nd ed. Washington, D.C.: US Government Printing Office; 2000; 2 November.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Ad Hoc Committee on Health Literacy for the Council on Scientific Affairs, American Medical Association. Health literacy: Report of the council on scientific affairs. J Am Med Assoc. 1999; 281: 552–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Nutbeam D. Health promotion glossary. Health Promot Int. 1998; 13: 349–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Golbeck AL, Ahlers-Schmidt CR, Paschal AM, Dismuke SE. A definition and operational framework for health numeracy. Am J Prev Med. 2005; 29: 375–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ancker JS, Kaufman D. Rethinking health numeracy: A multidisciplinary literature review. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2007; 14: 713–721.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ. The importance of mathematics in health and human judgment: Numeracy, risk communication, and medical decision making. Learn Indiv Differ. 2007; 17: 147–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Peters E, Vastfjall D, Slovic P, et al. Numeracy and decision making. Psychol Sci. 2006; 17: 408–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hibbard JH, Peters E, Dixon A, Tusler M. Consumer competencies and the use of comparative quality information: It isn’t just about literacy. Med Care Res Rev. 2007; 64: 379–394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kutner M, Greenberg E, Baer J. A First Look at the Literacy of Americas Adults in the 21st Century (NCES 2006-470). US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, D.C.: US Government Printing Office; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Williams MV, Parker RM, Baker DW, et al. Inadequate functional health literacy among patients at two public hospitals. J Am Med Assoc. 1995; 274: 1677–1682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Rothman RL, Housam R, Weiss H, et al. Patient understanding of food labels: The role of literacy and numeracy. Am J Prev Med. 2006; 31: 391–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Davis TC, Crouch MA, Long SW, et al. Rapid assessment of literacy levels of adult primary care patients. Fam Med. 1991; 23: 433–435.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Wilkinson G. WRAT-3: Wide Range Achievement Test, Administration Manual. Wilmington: Wide Range; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Black WC, Welch HG. The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Ann Intern Med. 1997; 127: 966–972.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Sheridan SL, Pignone MP, Lewis CL. A randomized comparison of patients’ understanding of number needed to treat and other common risk reduction formats. J Gen Intern Med. 2003; 18: 884–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Forrow L, Taylor WC, Arnold RM. Absolutely relative: How research results are summarized can affect treatment decisions. Am J Med. 1992; 92: 121–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sheridan SL, Pignone M. Numeracy and the medical students’ ability to interpret data. Eff Clin Pract. 2002; 5: 35–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lipkus IM, Samsa G, Rimer BK. General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Med Decis Mak. 2001; 21: 37–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Yamigishi K. When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: Implications for risk communication. Appl Cogn Psychol. 1997; 11: 495–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Reyna VF, Lloyd FJ, Whalen P. Genetic testing and medical decision making. Arch Intern Med. 2001; 161: 2406–2408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Malenka DJ, Baron JA, Johansen S, Wahrenberger JW, Ross JM. The framing effect of relative and absolute risk. J Gen Intern Med. 1993; 8: 543–548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Chao C, Studts JL, Abell T, et al. Adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer: How presentation of recurrence risk influences decision making. J Clin Oncol. 2003; 21: 4299–4305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Naylor CD, Chen E, Strauss B. Measured enthusiasm: Does the method of reporting trial results alter perceptions of therapeutic effectiveness. Ann Intern Med. 1992; 117: 916–921.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Edwards A, Elwyn G, Covey J, Matthews E, Pill R. Presenting risk information: A review of the effects of “framing” and other manipulations on patient outcomes. J Health Commun. 2001; 6: 61–682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Bucher HC, Weinbacher M, Gyr K. Influence of method of reporting study results on decision of physicians to prescribe drugs to lower cholesterol concentration. Br Med J. 1994; 309: 761–764.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Hux JE, Naylor CD. Communicating the benefits of chronic preventive therapy: Does the format of efficacy data determine patients’ acceptance of treatment? Med Decis Mak. 1995; 15: 152–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Sarfati D, Howden-Chapman P, Woodward A, Salmond C. Does the frame affect the picture? A study into how attitudes to screening for cancer are affected by the way benefits are expressed. J Med Screen. 1998; 5: 137–140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. DeWalt DA, Berkman ND, Sheridan S, Lohr KN, Pignone MP. Literacy and health outcomes: A systematic review of the literature. J Gen Intern Med. 2004; 19: 1228–1239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Berkman ND, DeWalt DA, Pignone MP, et al. Literacy and Health Outcomes. Summary, Evidence Report/Technology Assessment No. 87. Rockville: AHRQ; 2004. (Prepared by RTI International, University of North Carolina Evidence-based Practice Center under Contract No. 290-02-0016) AHRQ Publication No. 04-E007-1: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; January.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS, Nurss J. The relationship of patient reading ability to self-reported health and use of health services. Am J Pub Health. 1997; 87: 1027–1030.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Sentell TL, Halpin HA. Importance of adult literacy in understanding health disparities. J Gen Intern Med. 2006; 21: 862–866.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Williams MV, Baker DW, Parker RM, Nurss JR. Relationship of functional health literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease. Arch Intern Med. 1998; 158: 166–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. The test of functional health literacy in adults. J Gen Intern Med. 1995; 10: 537–541.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J. Development of a brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Educ Couns. 1999; 38: 33–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS. Health literacy and the risk of hospital admission. J Gen Intern Med. 1998; 13: 791–798.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Scott TL, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Baker DW. Health literacy and preventive health care use among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organization. Medical Care. 2002; 40: 395–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Estrada CA, Martin-Hryniewicz M, Peek BT, Collins C, Byrd JC. Literacy and numeracy skills and anticoagulation control. Am J Med Sci. 2004; 328: 88–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Apter AJ, Cheng J, Small D, et al. Asthma numeracy skill and health literacy. J Asthma. 2006; 43: 705–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Gazmararian JA, Kripalani S, Miller MJ, et al. Factors associated with medication refill adherence in cardiovascular-related diseases. J Gen Intern Med. 2006; 21: 1215–1221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lenert LA, Sherbourne CD, Reyna VF. Utility elicitation using single item questions compared with a computerized interview. Med Decis Mak. 2001; 21: 97–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Morimoto T, Fukui T. Utilities measured by rating scale, time trade-off, and standard gamble: Review and reference for health care professionals. J Epidemiol. 2002; 12: 160–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Woloshin S, Schwartz LM, Moncur M, Gabriel S, Tosteson ANA. Assessing values for health: Numeracy matters. Med Decis Mak. 2001; 21: 382–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Schwartz SR, McDowell J, Yueh B. Numeracy and the shortcomings of utility assessment in head and neck cancer patients. Head Neck. 2004; 26: 401–407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Lindau ST, Tomori C, Lyons T, et al. The association of health literacy with cervical cancer prevention knowledge and health behaviors in a multiethnic cohort of women. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2002; 186: 938–943.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Bass PF, Wilson JF, Griffith CH, Barnett DR. Residents’ ability to identify patients with poor literacy skills. Acad Med. 2002; 77: 1039–1041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Rogers ES, Wallace LS, Weiss BD. Misperceptions of medical understanding in low-literacy patients: Implications for cancer prevention. Cancer Control. 2006; 3: 225–229.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Doak LG, Doak CC. Patient comprehension profiles: Recent findings and strategies. Patient Couns Health Educ. 1980; 2: 101–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kicklighter JR, Stein MA. Factors influencing diabetic clients’ ability to read and comprehend printed diabetic diet material. Diabetes Educ. 1993; 19: 40–46.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Davis TC, Crouch MA, Wills G, Miller S, Abdehou DM. The gap between patient reading comprehension and the readability of patient education materials. J Family Pract. 1990; 31: 533–538.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Powers RD. Emergency department patient literacy and the readability of patient-directed materials. Ann Emerg Med. 1988; 17: 124–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Safeer RS, Keenan J. Health literacy: The gap between physicians and patients. Am Fam Phys. 2005; 72: 463–468.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Dunning D, Heath C, Suls JM. Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2004; 5: 69–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Welch HG. Can patients interpret health information? An assessment of the medical data interpretation test. Med Decis Mak. 2005; 25: 290–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Weiss BD, Mays MZ, Martz W, et al. Quick assessment of literacy primary care: The newest vital sign. Ann Fam Med. 2005; 3: 514–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Estrada C, Barnes V, Collins C, Byrd JC. Health literacy and numeracy. J Am Med Assoc. 1999; 282: 527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Montori VM, Leung TW, Thompson CA, et al. Validation of a diabetes numeracy evaluation tool [Abstract]. Diabetes. 2004; 53(Suppl 2): A224–A225.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Fagerlin A, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Ubel PA, et al. Measuring numeracy without a math test: Development of the subjective numeracy scale. Med Decis Mak. 2007; 27: 672–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Smith DM, Ubel PA, Fagerlin A. Validation of the subjective numeracy scale: Effects of low numeracy on comprehension of risk communications and utility elicitations. Med Decis Mak. 2007; 27: 663–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Woloshin S, Schwartz LM, Welch HG. Patients and medical statistics: Interest, confidence, and ability. J Gen Intern Med. 2005; 20: 996–1000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Feldman-Stewart D, Kocovski N, McConnell BA, et al. Perception of quantitative information for treatment decisions. Med Decis Mak. 2000; 20: 228–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Mazur DJ, Merz JF. How the manner of presentation of data influences older patients in determining their treatment preferences. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1993; 41: 223–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Ancker J, Senathirajah Y, Kukafka R, Starren J. Design features of graphs in health risk communication: A systematic review. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2006; 13: 608–618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Lipkus IM, Hollands JG. The visual communication of risk. J Natl Cancer Inst Monograph. 1999; 25: 149–163.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Cleveland W, McGill R. Graphical perception: Theory, experimentation, and application to the development of graphical methods. J Am Stat Assoc. 1984; 77: 541–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ. Numeracy, ratio bias and denominator neglect in judgments of risk and probability. Learn Individ Differ (in press, 2008).

  69. Reyna V, Brainerd C. Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learn Individ Differ. 1995; 7: 1–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Reyna VF. A theory of medical decision making and health: Fuzzy-trace theory. Med Decis Mak. (in press, 2008).

  71. Feldman-Stewart D, Brundage MD, Zotov V. Further insight into the perception of quantitative information: Judgments of gist in treatment decisions. Med Decis Mak. 2007; 27: 34–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Hollands JG, Tanaka T, Dyre BP. Understanding bias in proportion production. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2002; 28: 563–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Sandman P, Weinstein N, Miller P. High risk or low: How location on a “risk ladder” affected perceived risk. Risk Anal. 1994; 14: 35–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Lakoff G. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Fagerlin A, Ubel PA. What’s time got to do with it? Inattention to duration in interpretation of survival graphs. Risk Anal. 2005; 25: 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ. The origins of probability judgment: A review of data and theories. In: Wright G, Ayton P, eds. Subjective Probability. New York: Wiley; 1994: 239–272.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Stone E, Yates J, Parker A. Effects of numerical and graphical displays on professed risk-taking behavior. J Exp Psychol Appl. 1997; 3: 243–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Stone E, Sieck W, Bull B, et al. Foreground: Background salience: Explaining the effects of graphical displays on risk avoidance. Org Behav Hum Decis Process. 2003; 90: 19–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Schapira MM, Nattinger AB, McHorney CA. Frequency or probability? A qualitative study of risk communication formats used in health care. Med Decis Mak. 2001; 21: 459–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Zacks J, Levy E, Tversky B, Schiano D. Reading bar graphs: Effects of extraneous depth cues and graphical context. J Exp Psychol Appl. 1998; 4: 119–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Levy E, Zacks J, Tversky B, Schiano D. Gratuitous graphics? Putting preferences in perspective. Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. Vancouver: ACM; 1996: 42–49.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Fagerlin A, Zikmund-Fisher B, Ubel P, Smith D. Measuring numeracy and the impact of numeracy on medical decision making. Annual Meeting of the Society of Behavioral Medicine. Washington, DC; 2007.

  83. Peters E, Dieckmann N, Dixon A, Hibbard JH, Mertz CK. Less is more in presenting quality information to consumers. Med Care Res Rev. 2007; 64: 169–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Bastardi A, Shafir E. On the pursuit and misuse of useless information. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998; 75: 19–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. McNeil BJ, Pauker SG, Sox HC, Tversky A. On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. N Engl J Med. 1982; 306: 1259–1262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ. Fuzzy-trace theory and framing effects in choice: Gist extraction, truncation, and conversion. J Behav Decis Mak. 1991; 4: 249–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Benjamin DJ, Brown SA, Shapiro JM. “Who is “behavioral”? Cognitive ability and anomalous preferences. Available at: SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=675264. Accessibility verified December 17, 2007.

  88. Frederick S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J Econ Perspect. 2005; 19: 24–42.

    Google Scholar 

  89. Ashcraft MH. Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2002; 11: 181–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Fischhoff B. Assessing adolescent decision-making competence. Dev Rev (in press, 2008).

  91. Gigerenzer G. Why the distinction between single-event probabilities and frequencies is important for psychology (and vice versa). In: Wright G, Ayton P, eds. Subjective Probability. New York, NY: Wiley; 1994: 129–161.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Dehaene S. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Natter HM, Berry DC. Effects of active information processing on the understanding of risk information. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2005; 19: 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Chen MK, Lakshminaryanan V, Santos LR. The evolution of our preferences: Evidence from capuchin monkey trading behavior. J Polit Econ. 2006; 114: 517–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Barbey AK, Sloman SA. Base-rate respect: From ecological rationality to dual processes. Behav Brain Sci. 2007; 30: 241–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Sloman S. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol Bull. 1996; 119: 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Epstein S. Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. Am Psychol. 1994; 49: 709–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004; 24: 311–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Reyna VF, Mills BA. Converging evidence supports fuzzy-trace theory’s nested sets hypothesis (but not the frequency hypothesis). Behav Brain Sci. 2007; 30: 278–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Reyna VF, Ellis SC. Fuzzy-trace theory and framing effects in children’s risky decision making. Psychol Sci. 1994; 5: 275–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Reyna VF, Farley F. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2006; 7: 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Reyna VF, Lloyd FJ. Physician decision making and cardiac risk: Effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2006; 12: 179–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Reyna VF. Class inclusion, the conjunction fallacy, and other cognitive illusions. Dev Rev. 1991; 11: 317–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Reyna VF, Adam MB. Fuzzy-trace theory, risk communication, and product labeling in sexually transmitted diseases. Risk Anal. 2003; 23: 325–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Rivers S, Mills B, Reyna VF. Risky behavior under the influence: A fuzzy-trace theory of emotion in adolescence. Dev Rev (in press, 2008).

  106. Schapira M. The development of a health numeracy measure. Computer Retrieval of Information on Scientific Projects. Available at: http://crisp.cit.nih.gov. Accessibility verified December 13, 2007.

  107. Donelle L, Hoffman-Goetz L, Arocha JF. Assessing health numeracy among community-dwelling older adults. J Health Commun. 2007; 12: 651–655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Seligman HK, Wang FF, Palacios JL, et al. Physician notification of their diabetes patients’ limited health literacy: A randomized controlled trial. J Gen Intern Med. 2005; 20: 1001–1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Lipkus IM. Numeric, verbal, and visual formats of conveying health risks: Suggested best practices and future recommendations. Med Decis Mak. 2007; 27: 696–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Fagerlin A, Ubel PA. Mortality versus survival graphs: Improving temporal consistence in perceptions of treatment effectiveness. Patient Educ Couns. 2007; 66: 100–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Schapira MM, Nattinger AB, McAuliffe TL. The influence of graphic format on breast cancer risk communication. J Health Commun. 2006; 11: 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Stone ER, Yates JF, Parker AM. Risk communication: Absolute versus relative expressions of low-probability risks. Org Behav Hum Decis Process. 1994; 60: 387–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Carpenter PA, Shah P. A model of the perceptual and conceptual processes in graph comprehension. J Exp Psychol Appl. 1998; 4: 75–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Cleveland WS, McGill R. Graphical perception and graphical methods for analyzing scientific data. Science. 1985; 229: 828–833.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Burkell J. What are the chances? Evaluating risk and benefit information in consumer health materials. J Med Lib Assoc. 2004; 92: 200–208.

    Google Scholar 

  116. Wallsten TS, Budescu DV, Zwick R, Kemp SM. Preferences and reasons for communicating probabilistic information in verbal or numerical terms. Bull Psychon Soc. 1993; 31: 135–138.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Mazur DJ, Hickam DH. Patient interpretations of terms connoting low probabilities when communicating about surgical risk. Theor Surg. 1993; 8: 143–145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Kenney RM. Between never and always. N Engl J Med. 1981; 305: 1097–1098.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Bryant GD, Norman GR. Expressions of probability: Words and numbers. N Engl J Med. 1980; 302: 411–412.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Woloshin KK, Ruffin MT, Gorenflo DW. Patients’ interpretation of qualitative probability statements. Arch Fam Med. 1994; 3: 961–966.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Mazur DJ, Hickam DH, Mazur MD. How patients’ preferences for risk information influence treatment choice in a case of high risk and high therapeutic uncertainty: Asymptomatic localized prostate cancer. Med Decis Mak. 1999; 19: 394–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Mazur DJ, Hickam DH. Patients’ preferences for risk disclosure and role in decision making for invasive medical procedures. J Gen Intern Med. 1997; 12: 114–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Mazur DJ, Hickam DH. Patients’ interpretations of probability terms. J Gen Intern Med. 1991; 6: 237–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Brun W, Teigen KH. Verbal probabilities: Ambiguous, context-dependent, or both? Org Behav Hum Decis Process. 1988; 41: 390–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  125. O’Brien BJ. Words or numbers? The evaluation of probability expressions in general practice. J R Coll Gen Pract. 1989; 39: 98–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Kong A, Barnett GO, Mosteller F, Youtz C. How medical professionals evaluate expressions of probability. N Engl J Med. 1986; 315: 740–744.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Nakao MA, Axelrod S. Numbers are better than words: Verbal specifications of frequency have no place in medicine. Am J Med. 1983; 74: 1061–1065.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Mapes REA. Verbal and numerical estimates of probability in therapeutic contexts. Soc Sci Med. 1979; 13A: 277–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was based on a National Cancer Institute-sponsored symposium—Numeracy: A Critical (and Often Overlooked) Competence for Health Decision Making—presented at the Society of Behavioral Medicine Annual Meeting, Washington, D.C., USA, March 22, 2007. Dr. Reyna is supported by grants from the National Cancer Institute (R13CA126359) and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH-061211). Dr. Fagerlin is supported by an MREP early career award from the US Department of Veterans Affairs. Dr. Lipkus is supported by The Foundation for Informed Medical Decision Making. Dr. Peters is supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (SES-0517770). We thank Nathan Dieckmann for his helpful literature review.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wendy Nelson Ph.D..

About this article

Cite this article

Nelson, W., Reyna, V.F., Fagerlin, A. et al. Clinical Implications of Numeracy: Theory and Practice. ann. behav. med. 35, 261–274 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-008-9037-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-008-9037-8

Keywords

Navigation