Throughout history, infectious diseases have emerged and spread across regions or populations as epidemics. “Epidemics occurring over a very wide area, crossing international boundaries and usually affecting a large number of people” are known as pandemics (Porta, 2014, p.209). Pandemics affect societies through physical illness and mortality, as well as a range of disruptions, including economic, social, and political consequences (Qiu et al., 2017). In particular, pandemics related to viral respiratory infections (VRIs), such as influenza (e.g., H1N1) or coronaviruses (e.g., severe acute respiratory syndrome [SARS], COVID-19), tend to trigger higher anxiety among the general population due to its life-threatening and highly contagious nature (Saeed et al., 2020). In addition, given VRIs’ ease of transmission through air and water droplets (Ong et al., 2020), public health control measures for VRI-induced pandemics generally require mass lockdown of public spaces and quarantine of high-risk groups. Such measures further reinforce public fear and isolation (Ren et al., 2020), which may leave individuals or groups associated with VRIs (e.g., countries of origin, highly infected regions, frontliners, patients, caretakers) especially vulnerable to stigmatization.

From the perspective of cultural psychology, scholars have proposed that culture plays an important role in stigmatizing attitudes and experiences, due to its underlying moral cognition (Yang et al., 2007). While there have been some commentaries highlighting the effect of culture on stigmatization during pandemics (e.g., Biswas et al., 2021; Bruns et al., 2020), there is still a gap in the synthesis of evidence-based studies to understand the cultural determinants of stigma during pandemics. Our study aimed to bridge this gap by conducting a systematic review of the published empirical articles in the past 21 years, to understand the role of culture on the perception of stigma towards others and self during VRIs pandemic. We aimed to advocate for culturally responsive and sensitive community services during the post-pandemic era, especially among the marginalized minorities.

Stigma as social constructs

Stigmatization is defined as the differentiation and labeling of a collective group of individuals based on their characteristics, attributes, qualities, or status (Goffman, 1968). This usually leads stigmatized individuals or groups to experience social exclusion, discrimination, humiliation, and marginalization from the mainstream (e.g., Saeed et al., 2020). At the broadest level, stigma can be categorized into two distinct constructs, namely, public stigma and self-stigma (Corrigan & Watson, 2002). From a social psychological perspective, we adopted this broad definition of stigma in our systematic review, where public stigma refers to an individual expressing stigmatizing attitudes and behaviors towards others, and self-stigma is the perception of being stigmatized.

Public stigma

Public stigma is defined as the negative beliefs and attitudes one holds that lead to fear, avoidance, rejection, or discrimination towards individuals with the disadvantaged condition (Corrigan & Penn, 1999; Parcesepe & Cabassa, 2013). In the context of the pandemic, the publicly stigmatized populations may include infected persons or those perceived to be at a higher risk of being infected, such as healthcare workers, family members, or people of a particular nationality or ethnicity, through associative stigma (Bhanot et al., 2021; Duan et al., 2020; Goffman, 1968; Hoppe, 2018). Public stigma, especially in the context of infectious diseases, can be explained through the evolutionary perspective of “othering,” with its function to protect oneself and one’s in-groups (Kurzban & Leary, 2001). Stigmatizing others is viewed as an act of adapting to increase the chances of both individual and in-group survival from the “perceived threat”—infected persons, or individuals or groups perceived to have associated attributes with the disease. To the individual, the phenomenon of separating between “them” and “us” allows the idea of distancing from perceived threat; the label “them” temporarily induces feelings of security and reduces fear and anxiety (Gilmore & Somerville, 1994; Saeed et al., 2020). This is especially prominent as epidemics and pandemics are characterized by and exacerbated by fear of the disease and uncertainty (Ren et al., 2020; Saeed et al., 2020). Hence, pandemics and virus outbreaks would be expected to be accompanied by social stigma toward others.

Self-stigma

In this study, self-stigma refers to the awareness and perceptions of negative attitudes, prejudice, or discrimination others hold towards them (Corrigan & Rao, 2012; Link, 1987). The self-stigma model proposed by Corrigan and Rao (2012) argues that self-stigma may extend beyond initial awareness and perception of receiving stigma, to individuals internalizing those beliefs, leading to lower self-esteem. Sullivan et al. (2014) explained that scapegoating may occur in response to uncontrollable situations, usually towards the minority out-group, with the displacement of anger toward tangible and controllable targets in response to the uncontrollable situation. This is obvious during the pandemic, when the infected or “associated” people were blamed and discriminated against (e.g., Yang et al., 2021). As such, self-stigma usually results from the public projection of fear during the pandemic, where the stigmatized group (either infected or associated) becomes the scapegoat and the out-group. For example, during the COVID-19 pandemic, some of the self-stigma experiences ranged from feeling ashamed, being blamed, and being rejected, isolated, and abandoned by close ones, as well as the larger community (Duan et al., 2020; Nursalam et al., 2020; Yuan et al., 2021). Effects of self-stigma can also be long-term, extending beyond surviving the disease and even the pandemic itself. For example, SARS survivors struggled against stigma from family and co-workers (Lee et al., 2005), and those who were COVID-19 positive or quarantined reported self-stigma (e.g., feeling abandoned) even after recovery (Lohiniva et al., 2021; Nursalam et al., 2020). Hence, it is important to study the nature and mechanism behind self-stigma during both the pandemic and the recovery phase.

Culture and stigma

On the socio-political level, previous research has identified several determinants that are associated with public stigma and self-stigma, including sociodemographic correlates such as education, health literacy, socio-economic status, gender, age, race/ethnicity, exposure to the virus, occupation as a healthcare worker, or the media (see, e.g., Yuan et al., 2021). We argue that culture is an additional factor that warrants a systematic study due to how the mechanism of culture interplays with pandemic stigma. While Link et al. (2004) viewed stigma as a universal phenomenon across different cultures and societies, Yang et al. (2007) highlighted that the experience and the endorsement of stigmatization could be very different and distinct, as stigmatizing actions, reasons, and outcomes differ based on culture. Moreover, some ethnic/racial identities or nationalities that are perceived to have a higher likelihood of being the carrier of virus or are believed to be the regional origin of the disease are highly stigmatized. (e.g., Bhanot et al., 2021; Hoppe, 2018). For example, literature reported that pandemics such as SARS and COVID-19 have been mainly associated with East Asians or Chinese, especially through media portrayal such as the news (e.g., He et al., 2020; Peprah & Gyasi, 2021).

On an individual level, there is some evidence that a person’s socio-cultural traits and norms may affect their perceptions and coping with a disease or illness. For example, Ji et al. (2020) reported how mainland Chinese had a more optimistic outlook towards the pandemic and the construct of suffering compared to the Euro-Canadians, potentially explained through dialectical thinking, whereby happiness and suffering co-exist in the Chinese population. Guan et al. (2020) also further explained how cultural constructs such as cultural orientation (e.g., collectivism/individualism—Hofstede, 1980; Triandis, 1995), cultural norms (e.g., tight/loose society—Gelfand et al., 2011), cultural-prone thinking styles (e.g., holistic/analytic—Nisbett et al., 2001) and self-construal (independent/ interdependent—Markus & Kitayama, 1991) impacted coping during the COVID-19 pandemic. Together, these different studies and approaches point to the close relationship between culture and perception of the pandemic, from the individual level to the national level, which warrants further studies from the cultural perspective (e.g., Yu et al., 2021).

Research aims

In this current review, we defined “culture” as a group of people sharing the same membership, such as ethnicity, race, language, and religion, as well as having shared beliefs, values, social norms, ideologies, traditions, or customs (Ferraro, 1998; Hofstede, 1991; Matsumoto et al., 1996). To date, a few published commentaries have attempted to describe the role of culture on public stigma during the COVID-19 pandemic, such as discriminating against or blaming a specific cultural group (e.g., Biswas et al., 2021; Bruns et al., 2020). While certain cultural groups are more vulnerable to social rejection during a pandemic, there is a gap about what are the prominent cultural determinants of the exhibition of stigma towards the “culturally other”. Moreover, as both culture and pandemic stigma are complex and multifaceted constructs, measures of these constructs are not consistent in the existing literature. A systematic review is ideal for synthesizing findings through an operationalized definition of these constructs to summarize how culture may influence stigma, with culture as the major theme or independent variable, and pandemic stigma (both self-stigma and public stigma) as the outcome or dependent variable. This type of review is needed to prevent or mitigate pandemic stigma in the future, by designing culturally responsive health campaigns and policies. Therefore, we conducted a systematic review that aimed to examine cultural factors associated with public stigma and self-stigma during VRI pandemics.

Method

Search strategy

We conducted a rigorous systematic review following PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009) in the following electronic databases: OVID (including APA PsycArticles Full Text, APA PsycINFO), Scopus, and all databases in Ebscohost (including CINAHL), with criteria set to include peer-reviewed full-text journals in English, from January 2000 to the first week of March 2022, with human participants. The search terms for the three keywords, 1) culture, 2) stigma, and 3) pandemic, were brainstormed and discussed between the authors based on the thesaurus and online searches, as shown in Table 1. We adopted wider definitions of all these three key terms to be exhaustive in our initial search, especially the types of pandemics being researched.

Table 1 Search Terms for Main Keywords and Databases Used

Study selection and eligibility criteria

Only studies fulfilling the following criteria were included:

  • (1) Full-text articles in the English language.

  • (2) Publications in peer-reviewed journals online from January 2000 to the third week of March 2022.

  • (3) Studies with human participants (i.e., content analysis of social media or news were excluded).

  • (4) Empirical studies—quantitative, qualitative, or mixed-methods (authors’ views, commentaries, theoretical papers, systematic reviews or meta-analyses, book chapters, conference proceedings, dissertations, and theses were excluded).

  • (5) Studies examining culture as an independent variable and stigma as a dependent variable in the context of a pandemic.

  • (6) Studies of viral respiratory infectious disease at pandemic or epidemic level only (i.e., coronavirus or influenza) (non-respiratory pandemic/epidemic such as Human Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV) or Ebola infections were excluded).

The screening process was reported in detail as per Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) reporting guidelines (Moher et al., 2009), as shown in Fig. 1. The initial literature search yielded 70,737 research articles across the 3 databases: Ovid (N = 38,966), Ebscohost (N = 13,342) and Scopus (N = 18,429). The citations and abstracts were first imported into Endnote X9, which were further imported into the online Covidence platform to remove duplicates. After removing duplicates in Covidence, 50,251 research articles were left for further screening. The research team (consisting of the first and fourth authors and six research assistants) screened each title and abstract for relevance to the systematic review based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria, and excluded 49,932 articles in the preliminary screening (most of them were non-VRI diseases such as HIV). The team then reviewed the remaining 319 articles in full text where 286 articles were further excluded due to not fulfilling the screening criteria above.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) Flow Chart of Systematic Review

Critical appraisal of the selected articles

The remaining 33 articles were further reviewed independently by the first and third authors, using Hawker’s critical appraisal ratings (Hawker et al., 2002), and rated as very poor, poor, fair, or good (score 1 to 4) for each of the following nine sections: abstract and title, introduction and aims, method and data, sampling, data analysis, ethics and bias, findings/results, transferability/generalizability, and implications and usefulness (see Table 2). The ratings from the initial review were averaged, and the discrepancy was reconciled by the second author. Two studies were excluded due to poor quality in their methodology (less than the cut-off score of 21, max = 36). Finally, 31 studies were included for the final synthesis in this study.

Table 2 Hawker’s Score for Critical Appraisal of Articles (Averaged Scores from 2 Independent Raters with Likert Scale from 1–4)

Qualitative coding of the findings

The above 31 articles were coded using a bottom-up inductive method through thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006) by the first and second authors. Next, the themes were discussed and agreed upon by all authors following a consensual qualitative research (CQR) approach, using bottom-up coding (Hill et al., 2005) to minimize individual biases in interpretation, based on the research question.

Results

Study characteristics

As seen in Table 3, the majority of the studies were conducted in North America (n = 19), with 16 from the United States of America (USA), two from Canada, and one from the USA and Canada. Others came from diverse regions in Europe (n = 3), Asia (n = 6), Australia (n = 1), and South Africa (n = 1). One study included 173 countries worldwide, with regional comparisons. Most studies were conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic (n = 29), with the rest conducted during SARS (n = 1) or H1N1 (n = 1). Studies relied on quantitative (n = 17), qualitative (n = 10), and mixed (n = 4) methods. Most studies examined self-stigma (n = 26), while only seven examined public stigma (n = 7) as the outcome variable/s. Other details, including participants’ characteristics and measurements, are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 Characteristics of the 31 Selected Articles for Systematic Review

Cultural factors and pandemic-related public stigma

From the seven studies that examined public stigma (e.g., discriminatory attitudes), four cultural factors were identified: Race or Ethnicity, Religious Identity, Collectivistic Values, and Residence in Non-Western Countries (see Table 4).

Table 4 Cultural Factors Associated with Public Stigma (PS) during Pandemics caused by Respiratory Viruses

[Insert Table 4: Cultural Factors Associated with Public Stigma during Pandemics].

Race and ethnicity

As shown in Table 4, three studies found that being members of the majority race or ethnicity was associated with public stigma. In Wong and Sam’s study (2011), ethnicity was associated with the extent to which participants endorsed stigmatizing attitudes during the H1N1 pandemic. They found that among Malaysians, Malays endorsed the highest public stigma in the domain of fear (such as being afraid of those from overseas or eating outside) compared to Chinese and Indians. A qualitative study by Schmidt et al. (2020) also found that ethnic or racial identity was associated with public stigma towards other ethnicities during the COVID-19 pandemic, whereby some of the Blacks in South Africa believed COVID-19 to be a “White-mans’ disease” or a disease for those in China or Italy. They believed that the disease only affected “… upper-class people who travel a lot”, “it was for the White people” or that “it doesn’t affect Black people, we have strong genes…” (Schmidt et al., 2020, p.12). On the other hand, a study in China by Jiang et al. (2021) found that ethnic minorities expressed higher public stigma, such as fear and avoidance towards COVID-19 patients and people from Wuhan, compared to the Han majority. Overall, these studies demonstrated that racial and ethnic identities were associated with negative perceptions of others who were affiliated with the pandemic, especially those from other racial groups.

Religious identity

Two studies found different religious identities within a country were associated with differences in public stigma during the COVID-19. In Lebanon, those who identified as a Muslim or a Druze reported higher discriminatory attitudes towards those infected with COVID-19 compared to the Christians (Haddad et al., 2021). A study in India by Islam et al. (2021) found that while Hindus perceived Muslims to be responsible for the spread of the COVID-19, Muslims did not hold this perception towards the Hindus.

Collectivistic values

Findings from Ahuja et al. (2020) showed a positive relationship between collectivism and pandemic-related public stigma in India, where collectivism was defined as the extent to which family and society needs take importance over an individual’s needs (Kim et al., 2016). They found that the more the non-Muslim participants endorsed collectivist values, the more they expressed public stigma (blaming and attributing the spread of the COVID-19 pandemic) towards Muslims in India.

Non-Western countries

A global survey conducted by Dye et al. (2020) across 173 countries found differences in COVID-19 related public stigma across different geographical regions. In comparison to participants from Europe, Northern America, or Oceania, participants from Non-western countries such as Asia, Africa, Latin America, and Caribbean regions were more likely to believe that individuals in their community had a negative perception of those who were infected or associated with COVID-19. They were also more likely to believe that people lost status and respect in the community when they tested COVID-19 positive compared to participants in Europe, Northern America, or Oceania. However, Dye and colleagues (2020) did not explain why there were differences across the regions.

Cultural factors and pandemic-related self-stigma

Out of 26 articles, we identified five cultural factors which had self-stigma during the pandemic (e.g., perceived stigma/discrimination, harassment) as the outcome variable: Minority by Race, such as the overseas Chinese, Minority by Religion, Minority in Spoken Language, Cultural Values that conflicted with the Dominant Society, and Countries in Northern America, Africa, Oceania, and Asian regions (see Table 5).

Table 5 Cultural Factors Associated with Self-Stigma (SS) during Pandemics caused by Respiratory Viruses

[Insert Table 5: Cultural factors Associated with Self-Stigma related to Pandemics].

Ethnic Minorities, especially Chinese in Western Countries

This was the most frequently identified cultural factor (N = 23). In North America (the USA and Canada), several studies found that Asians reported the highest degree of being stigmatized or discriminated against during the COVID-19 pandemic compared to other races, especially Whites (Inman et al., 2021; Miconi et al., 2021a, 2021b; Pan et al., 2021) and other People of Color (POC), such as Blacks or Latino/a (Molock & Parchem, 2021), or Native Hawaiians and Pacific Islanders (Park et al., 2022). Generally, in the US, Asians were seen to experience higher self-stigma compared to non-Asians during the COVID-19 pandemic (Kim et al., 2022). Moreover, some Asians attributed self-stigma to their ethnicity or race. Dye et al. (2020) reported that across 173 countries Asian and Chinese participants felt stigmatized during the COVID-19 pandemic due to their ethnicity. For instance, a Southeast Asian participant in Europe stated, “I am Asian…people treated me badly and avoided me.” (Dye et al., 2020, p.10). Similar findings were echoed among Chinese or Asians in France, and the US, whereby their Asian physical appearance, as well as China being the pandemic origin, were some of the reasons for feeling stigmatized and being discriminated against during the COVID-19 pandemic (Ji & Chen, 2022; Oh & Min, 2022; Wang et al., 2021). Interestingly, Ji and Chen (2022) indicated that the Chinese international students not only felt stigmatized by the majority in the US, but also felt blamed and received less support from the Chinese back in China, as they were told it was their own choice and responsibility for leaving China and studying in the US.

Moreover, self-stigma during the pandemic was not limited to Asians per se, as other ethnic minorities also reported higher experiences of discrimination compared to the majority in the USA (e.g., Le et al., 2022, Shah et al., 2021, Strassle et al., 2022, & Tan & Umamaheswar, 2021). For instance, ethnic minorities such as Blacks and South Asians in the USA reported similar narratives where they felt that they were stigmatized by the public and the community around them such as, “I think somehow there’s always this narrative that we’re the other…We have COVID, and nobody else seems to have it. And we’re the ones that are spreading it around.” (Vandrevala et al., 2022, p.146). Interestingly, not only were particular racial labels associated with intensified self-stigma, but personal identification with the racial label was found to be associated with a higher level of self-stigma. For instance, Zong et al. (2021) found that those Chinese American adolescents who identified themselves most as “Chinese” (participation in Chinese culture) and lowest as “American” reported the highest level of racial discrimination experiences, compared to their counterparts who identified either as bicultural or had a low level of identification with both cultures. Increased self-stigma among ethnic minorities was not limited to COVID-19 pandemic, as an earlier qualitative study during SARS also found that residents of the Chinatown in New York, USA reported feeling stigmatized due to their ethnicity (Eichelberger, 2007), such as a participant stated: “I did feel more self-conscious that I was getting these looks because I was—I am Asian, Asian–American. That was hard for me.” (p. 1290). In short, the studies showed that ethnic identity, especially being the minority, was associated with higher self-stigma during a pandemic.

Religious identity, especially among the minorities

Three studies found that minority religious groups were stigmatized during the COVID-19 pandemic. For example, in Quebec, Canada, individuals who self-reported as “other” religion reported the highest discrimination during the COVID-19 pandemic, followed closely by Islam and then Christianity, Atheism, and Judaism (Miconi et al., 2021a). Moreover, in Dye and colleagues’ (2020) study, a Muslim participant wrote that they felt stigmatized in a South Asian country, “As I belong to a particular religion [Islam], the people near my home keep on bullying us for the coronavirus, which is not fair” (Dye et al., 2020, p.10). Weiden et al. (2021) found that a third of the participants from the cultural minority group in Israel, Ultra-orthodox Jews, perceived they experienced microaggressions due to their religious identity during the COVID-19 pandemic. Interestingly, Haddad et al. (2021) found that Muslims in Lebanon (the majority) who were diagnosed with COVID-19 were more likely to report feeling isolated by others or having higher internalized shame concerning their condition compared to Christians who were diagnosed.

Being a minority by spoken language

Self-stigma was not only associated with religious minorities but also with those who were a minority by spoken language. In the same study conducted in Quebec, Canada, Miconi et al. (2021a) found that those who were English-speaking were more likely to report experiences of discrimination during COVID-19 compared to those who were bilingual (English and French) or French-speaking only. As Quebec is a French dominant region, this implies that those who could not speak the French language (most likely a foreigner) faced higher self-stigma than others during the pandemic.

Cultural values conflict with the dominant society

A qualitative study by Ma and Zhan (2020) found that a mismatch in cultural values was associated with stigma experiences. They interviewed Chinese international students who felt stigmatized by Americans in the USA. Chinese students reported that they were being stigmatized due to the mismatch in cultural values and norms between the Chinese and mainstream American society, which resulted in differences in health behaviors and beliefs during the pandemic. For instance, an interviewee mentioned, “American culture values human rights and freedom. The American people do not listen to their government, even if their government asks them to do something. Chinese people tend to listen.” and another, “…Americans look…to me…they live a very independent and individualistic life…” (Ma & Zhan, 2020, p.14). Another participant quoted their American Professor’s judgment of them for wearing masks, “Why are you wearing masks? You are selfish. You should leave the masks to doctors” (Ma & Zhan, 2020, p.11).

Countries and regions

Finally, one study found that self-reported discrimination differed among countries. Countries from African, Asian, Northern American, and Oceania regions reported higher experiences of self-stigma, represented by COVID-19 related bullying/discrimination, compared to those in Latin America and Europe (Dye et al., 2020).

Discussion

The current systematic review identified a number of cultural factors associated with public stigma and self-stigma during pandemics caused by VRI. Previous researchers have highlighted that stigma is a complex and multi-faceted phenomenon, with factors influencing stigma at different levels, including culture (e.g., Link & Phelan, 2001). Erez and Gati (2004) illustrated the intersection of a multi-level culture model from individual level to group, organizational, national to global levels. However, there has been no integrative model of how culture at different levels interact with stigma specifically. Hence, based on our findings, we attempted to construct the cultural factors we identified into a multi-level systemic model of cultural levels relevant to pandemic stigma. This consisted of culture from the micro-individual domain, meso-level shared cultural group identity, and the larger macro-society/ecology one resides in. The micro domain describes the influence of culture at the individual level, such as cultural values or beliefs. Next, at the meso-level of shared identity, we identified cultural group identities such as race/ethnicity, religion, or shared language, which influenced pandemic stigma. Lastly, the influence of culture at the regional or country level was mapped onto the macro-level domain, termed cultural ecology. We label this model tentatively as a “systemic cultural stigma model” (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Systemic Cultural Stigma Model. Note: The Systemic Cultural Stigma Model displays factors associated with Public Stigma on the left side and factors associated with Self-Stigma on the right side

Here, we discuss this cultural stigma model in relation to both public stigma and self-stigma during the pandemic from the perspective of evolutionary cognition from Strong Ties Weak Ties (STWT) cultural rationality theory (Sundararajan, 2020; Yeh et al., 2022) and social group conflicts from scapegoating theory (Allport, 1954). Then, we discuss the implications of our findings for community healthcare.

Cultural domains and public stigma

As shown in the systemic cultural stigma model in Fig. 2, collectivistic values (individual level), ethnic/racial and religious identity (shared cultural identity), and non-western countries (cultural ecology) were associated with pandemic-related public stigma. These cultural factors could be interpreted through cultural niche-rationality theory, recently proposed by Sundararajan ‘s (2020). Cultural rationality theory posits how individuals differ in their cognition, such as reasoning about the world based on their evolutionary ecology, which influences the subsequent choice of survival behaviors (Morfett, 2018; Sundararajan, 2020). A Strong-Ties based community is described as comprised of close-knit relationships of family or kinship, with a cognitive orientation to external cues and resources (i.e., relying on strong-ties networks during hardship, shared responsibilities, and decision making), whereas weak-ties networks refer to distant relationship networks, such as acquaintances or strangers (Sundararajan, 2020; Ting & Sundararajan, 2018). With this foundation, STWT cultural rationality theory posits that an individual with a more dominant strong-ties rationality is more holistic in their cognitive style, where they perceive self in relation to those around them, aligning with the concept of related others (Sundararajan, 2020). Hence, in strong-ties dominant societies, individuals prioritize safety and wellness of family, kinship, and close friends, with fewer considerations given to those perceived as threats (regardless of in-group out-group membership), which could result in the social rejection of the latter. Values such as collectivism are privileged among strong-ties dominant societies, while individualism is privileged among weak-ties dominant societies (Sundararajan & Yeh, 2021). Research has also identified some associations between certain religions or ethnicities that historically embody kinship and bloodline as important and their members being more dominant in strong-ties rationality (Thong et al., 2022; Ting & Zhang, 2021; Yeh et al., 2022).

Public stigma, especially in the context of infectious diseases, has previously been explained through evolutionary needs, with its function to preserve group survival by distancing from those who are perceived as threats, namely the out-groups (Gilmore & Somerville, 1994; Kurzban & Leary, 2001; Saeed et al., 2020). During disasters such as pandemics, a society becomes tighter in its social norms, expecting higher conformity from its members, due to the commonly perceived threat of a contagious virus (Gelfand et al., 2021). STWT cultural rationality theory (Sundararajan, 2020) further suggests this type of reasoning is more privileged by strong-ties societies, following the evolutionary discourse, where tribal survival needs are prioritized over individual survival.

We found that the cultural factors associated with public stigma represented characteristics of strong-ties rationality at the micro-level through to strong-ties society at the macro-level. At the individual level, one study showed an association between collectivistic values and public stigma (Ahuja et al., 2020). At the group identity level, strong-ties ethnic identity was seen to have led to the social rejection of others who were more susceptible to the virus (Jiang, et al., 2021; Schmidt et al., 2020). In addition, we also found that religious groups in strong-ties societies were associated with higher public stigma toward the perceived threat, COVID-19 infected or affiliated persons (Haddad et al., 2021; Islam et al., 2021). Lastly, at the cultural ecology level, a study found higher public stigma among strong-ties countries, such as in Asia, Africa, Latin America, and the Caribbean regions compared to the weak-ties countries such as Europe, Northern America, or Oceania (Dye et al., 2020). The former regions have historically been considered as collectivistic societies in previous cross-cultural psychology literatures (Eaton & Louw, 2000; Holloway, 1990; Triandis, 2015). Past literature (e.g., Krendl & Pescosolido, 2020) on mental illness stigma has also corroborated our findings demonstrating a higher endorsement of stigma in Eastern countries (strong-ties characteristics) even towards their own in-group members due to the perceived threat to their group dynamics compared to Western countries (weak-ties characteristics). Overall, evolutionary cognition and niche could well explain why these strong-ties related cultural features are associated with public stigma, due to their association with the conservation of bloodline and in-group survival.

Culture domains and self-stigma

As shown in our cultural stigma model, having conflicting or mismatching cultural values with the dominant society (individual level), or being a minority by ethnicity, religion, or language (shared cultural identity), or particular countries (cultural ecology) were cultural factors associated with pandemic-related self-stigma. We propose this pattern of self-stigma being perceived primarily among the minorities could be explained using scapegoating theory (e.g., Allport, 1954; Sullivan et al., 2014). According to Allport (1954), during social turbulence, “scapegoating” occurs as people displace their anger and frustration on those perceived as being minority group members, usually based on common cultural labels such as ethnicity or religion. Sullivan et al. (2014) further posited that the mechanism of displacing blame on tangible and controllable targets when situations are perceived as out of control (e.g., a crisis) brings a temporary sense of control to those of the majority. Hence, during a disastrous pandemic, this could result in minority cultural groups being the scapegoats, subject to self-stigma. We found that the majority of the articles in our review pointed to Chinese or Asian Americans as the ethnic minority group who felt most stigmatized or discriminated against during the pandemic due to their behavior of wearing a mask (Ma & Zhan, 2020), their physical appearance, or their affiliation with countries in the Asian region where the pandemic was believed to have originated (e.g., Dye et al., 2020; Eichelberger, 2007; Miconi et al., 2021a, b; Molock & Parchem, 2021; Oh & Min, 2022). Moreover, other ethnic minorities, such as Persons of Color in the US, also reported higher self-stigma, potentially as the result of scapegoating (e.g., Le et al. 2022; Tan & Umamaheswar, 2021). Other forms of cultural identity were also associated with higher self-stigma and discrimination during the pandemic, such as those who were members of a minority by religion or spoken language (Miconi et al., 2021a, b; Weiden et al., 2021). Especially in the context of religion, both dominant and non-dominant religious groups reported higher self-stigma (e.g., Haddad et al., 2021, Islam et al., 2021), suggesting religious identity is susceptible to self-stigma during a pandemic regardless of their social status. Lastly, in the larger ecology domain, one study found differences in the level of self-stigma across countries in different regions (Dye et al., 2020).

Implications of our study for communal well-being

Our findings regarding cultural factors associated with stigma during the pandemic led to the proposed cultural stigma model introduced above. Despite the need for more research in certain cultural domains, our proposed model may guide the design of public health interventions during the recovery stage of the pandemic. We suggest national policies should consider culturally-responsive approaches to increase the effectiveness of anti-stigma campaigns in the community. For instance, in strong-ties societies, policymakers could reframe anti-stigma interventions as “communal thriving”, which promotes the idea of “togetherness”. To overcome disproportionate fear and perceived threat of marginalized groups, government agencies and Non-Governmental Organizations (NGOs) could emphasize messages and awareness campaigns on practical and tangible methods of keeping safe as a whole community regardless of group membership during the pandemic for the larger good (e.g., “We are all one family” or “We are all in the same boat”). On the other hand, since cultural minority groups in general perceived higher self-stigma, national bodies such as the health ministry through national policies or media could advocate for their pandemic control campaigns to be more culturally responsive towards less-represented groups (e.g., ethnic minorities, migrant workers, refugees). Moreover, during future pandemics, healthcare providers (e.g., public health officials, mental health professionals) could focus on increasing awareness of the scapegoating phenomena, introducing cross-cultural empathy exercises, and increasing the internal locus of control within individuals in the weak-ties society, to help reduce displaced anger and frustration toward minority groups. Overall, with more culturally responsive health campaigns, we anticipate a better quality of life at the individual level, as well as positive community-wide benefits, with fewer instances of isolation and division within the society.

Gaps in the literature and recommendations for future studies

Our review highlighted that there is limited rigorous and empirical literature exploring the role of culture in pandemic-related stigma. While differences in cultural characteristics such as ethnocultural identity associated with public stigma or self-stigma were reported, cultural theories explaining those observed differences were absent. In addition, the majority of the reviewed articles only used self-reported cultural characteristics. There is a need for researchers to also use cultural scales to assess participants’ underlying cultural values (e.g., collectivism/individualism), rationality (i.e., STWT), thinking styles, etc. Moreover, the self-reported cultural characteristics, such as ethnicities, were compared with unequal sample sizes which, while reflective of the population in those countries, may limit the power of the analyses. As the current review led to our proposed cultural stigma model (see Fig. 2), we recommend that future studies consider assessing all three domains to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the role of culture. Specifically, more studies could involve cultural measures in both the individual and ecology domains, to examine the dynamic interaction between the different cultural domains. There were also limited studies examining public stigma, as the majority studied only self-stigma during the pandemic. Understanding both constructs of stigma is important, as the systematic review showed that the cultural factors associated with each differed. Furthermore, there were multiple stigma scales adopted in the different studies, which only tapped into certain aspects of stigma, such as fear (e.g., Wong & Sam, 2011) or discriminatory treatment (e.g., Dye et al., 2020), making comparisons across studies difficult. Some scales were only validated in one single national sample. Hence, we also recommend either exploring stigma concepts that may be specific to a culture through a bottom-up approach, or developing and validating standardized pandemic stigma scales for future research that are suitable for cross-cultural comparisons.

Limitations of this study

As this systematic review only included journals published in English, it is possible we may have missed indigenous or international papers published in other languages. Moreover, due to the limited representation of countries (e.g., most studies were from the USA), there is a need to exercise caution in interpreting the current review, as there is a limit to the generalizability of the findings. Regarding the systemic cultural stigma model, there was only one study each that mapped onto the cultural values and ecology domain, due to the limited availability of empirical studies in these cultural domains relating to pandemic stigma. Lastly, as this study only reviewed cultural factors and their association to stigma, other possible contributing factors to stigma, such as sociodemographic variables (e.g., age, gender, SES, political affiliation) or personal variables (beliefs and motivation), have not been taken into consideration.

Conclusion

The findings from our systematic review highlighted the interplay between culture and pandemic stigma, as well as informing implications for interventional campaigns at multicultural societies. We examined the intersectionality of three cultural domains, individual cultural values, cultural identity, and cultural ecology, and incorporated STWT cultural rationality theory and scapegoating theory, to draw on both evolutionary and sociological perspectives to explain how culture may affect pandemic stigma. We also highlighted the need for more mixed-method research in non-Western societies to look at both types of stigma. Finally, we advocate that it is essential to promote culturally-responsive stigma interventions in the community, informed by research, to increase the well-being and inclusivity of minority groups during a pandemic.