Introduction

Tetris is a computer game where geometric blocks of different shapes and colors fall from the top of the screen in a random sequence one at a time. The task of the player is to rotate and place these blocks at the bottom of the screen. Fitting blocks together to complete horizontal lines is rewarded with points and extended game time. The immersive nature of Tetris gameplay, often capturing the player’s full attention towards the game, and its imagery-competing (e.g. visuospatial) aspects has recently been harnessed in the development of novel psychological interventions (Singh et al., 2020; Skorka-Brown et al., 2014). In particular, Tetris is being used as one component within psychological interventions to reduce dysfunctional mental imagery. A novel behavioral intervention, involving Tetris gameplay together with specific instructions to engage in mental rotation (i.e., as part of several intervention components), has shown promise to both prevent the development of intrusive memories (Horsch et al., 2017; Iyadurai et al., 2018; Kanstrup et al., 2021a) and reduce the frequency of long-standing intrusive memories after trauma (Kanstrup et al., 2021b; Kessler et al., 2018). Another procedure involving Tetris gameplay has been shown to reduce the strength, frequency, and vividness of dysfunctional cravings (e.g., alcohol, food, nicotine; Skorka-Brown et al., 2014; Skorka-Brown et al., 2015). Tetris has also been used within a procedure to reduce the emotional intensity and vividness of mental imagery during recall of emotional memories (Engelhard et al., 2010). Nevertheless, what makes Tetris a successful tool in psychological interventions is still incompletely understood, including what Tetris gameplay entails at the neural level.

Mental imagery refers to sensory experiences in the absence of external sensory input (Holmes & Agren, 2020). A prominent feature of mental imagery is that it provokes similar emotional (Holmes & Mathews, 2005), physiological and neural responses (Hoppe et al., 2021; Pearson, 2019) “as if” the imagined event was encountered in real life (e.g., imaging vs. seeing a spider). The impact of mental imagery on emotion is indisputably manifested in posttraumatic stress disorder in which distressing, intrusive mental imagery of past trauma constitutes a core symptom (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Evidence indicates that visual mental imagery hinges on visuospatial working memory (Baddeley & Andrade, 2000). For instance, taxing working memory resources with a concurrent visuospatial task reduces the vividness (liveliness and clarity) and emotional impact of mental imagery (Bywaters et al., 2004; Kavanagh et al., 2001). Tetris is hypothesized to act as a cognitive task that competes with mental imagery for working memory resources when, for example, an emotional memory is held in mind (e.g., Engelhard et al., 2010; Lau-Zhu et al., 2017; Skorka-Brown et al., 2014). That is, Tetris is hypothesised to place competition for limited working memory and can therefore act as an imagery-competing interference task to reduce the frequency or intensity of dysfunctional mental imagery.

The behavioral intervention that targets intrusive memories after trauma is based on the hypothesis that Tetris gameplay can interfere with the initial consolidation of the visuospatial mental imagery components of recent trauma memories (Deeprose et al., 2012) or the reconsolidation of already established, older trauma memories (James et al., 2015), by taxing visuospatial working memory specifically (Lau-Zhu et al., 2017). It is hypothesized that the active use of mental rotation during Tetris gameplay (i.e., visualizing the upcoming blocks in the game and rotating them in the mind’s eye to work out where they best fit) promotes the effects of the intervention by increasing mental imagery and visuospatial aspects of the way the game is played. Specific instructions emphasizing mental rotation is therefore thought to be one important component in protocols using this intervention (Iyadurai et al., 2018; Kanstrup et al., 2021a, b; Kessler et al., 2018). The use of Tetris as a visuospatial intervention to combat cravings is rooted in the elaborated intrusion (EI) theory (Kavanagh et al., 2005). EI considers cravings to be cognitive-emotional states involving embodied sensory imagery, and that a visuospatial interference task can compete with the resources used for the intrusive imagery and thus neutralize the initiated emotions of cravings (Skorka-Brown et al., 2014, 2015). Although interventions for intrusive memories and cravings emphasize that the usefulness of Tetris gameplay hinges on its capacity to compete with mental imagery and load visuospatial working memory specifically (but see Engelhard et al., 2010), little is known about the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these intervention procedures, and the neural basis of Tetris gameplay, such as its proposed visuospatial aspects, are not fully understood.

Tetris gameplay has previously been used in neuroimaging studies to investigate brain activity in emotion regulatory neural circuits (amygdala and occipital cortex; Price et al., 2013), to examine the impact of repeated practice of a visuospatial/motor task on overall brain activity (Haier et al., 1992; Rietschel et al., 2012), and to assess the plasticity of cortical grey matter (Haier et al., 2009). Also, a neuroimaging study on posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) showed that daily Tetris gameplay (3 months), in conjunction with psychological treatment, increased hippocampal volume, which was associated with PTSD symptom reduction (Butler et al., 2020). However, there is a need for further neuroimaging studies pinpointing the visuospatial aspects of Tetris gameplay. Brain areas involved in the visuospatial aspects of Tetris gameplay could be examined more specifically by controlling for the motor activity elicited by button presses during gameplay, and by using instructions emphasizing mental rotation during gameplay to boost mental imagery and visuospatial processing. Identifying the neural underpinnings of Tetris gameplay may help contribute to a mechanistic understanding of visuospatial interference tasks, which, in turn, could inform ways to optimize existing interventions and promote the development of other visuospatial interventions with therapeutic effects (Holmes et al., 2018; Singh et al., 2020).

Aim of the Study

The present study aimed to characterize brain areas involved in visuospatial aspects of Tetris gameplay, by controlling for motor activity (button presses), and using gameplay instructions emphasizing mental rotation during gameplay (in line with how it is used in emerging intervention protocols). To this end, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a subtraction design to identify brain areas activated during Tetris gameplay while controlling for motor activity. To mirror a clinical implementation of Tetris gameplay, we use the same instructions on mental rotation as those given within an intervention procedure targeting the reduction of intrusive memories after trauma (Iyadurai et al., 2018; Kanstrup et al., 2021a, b; Kessler et al., 2018;).

Methods and Materials

Participants

Thirty participants were recruited through advertisements on social media and billboards. Exclusion criteria consisted of current psychiatric disorder (other than spider phobia), substance abuse, neurological disease, receiving psychological treatment or psychotropic medication within six months, and contraindications for magnetic resonance imaging. Data was collected as part of an unrelated experiment where one of the inclusion criteria was fear of spiders (Hoppe et al., 2021). Twenty-eight participants (age: M = 23.8, SD = 5.8 years, 7 men and 21 women) were included in analyses. All participants, except for one, were right-handed. One participant was excluded from analyses as the participant did not follow the instructions, and another participant was excluded because of missing data. The study was approved by the national Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided written and informed consent. Participants were paid an amount corresponding to $ 50 for their participation. The data is available in the Open Science Framework [https://osf.io/qj8xc/].

Primary Outcome Measure

The primary outcome measure was brain activity, defined as blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal assessed with fMRI, during Tetris gameplay while controlling for motor activity.

Materials

For use in the fMRI scanner, an in-house Tetris app was programmed using the Unity graphics engine (version 5.2.3, Unity Technologies, San Francisco, CA). Two hand controls using pistol grip (Nordic NeuroLab, Bergen, Norway), with two buttons each (thumb and index finger), were used for gameplay in the scanner.

Procedure

Before entering the scanner, participants were instructed on how to play Tetris using active mental rotation (i.e., to visualize in their mind’s eye how to rotate/move the Tetris blocks to fit them into horizontal lines – planning ahead for the next block to appear on the screen during gameplay; see Supplementary materials for protocol). Left and right index fingers were used to steer the blocks left and right. The left thumb was used to rotate blocks, and the right thumb to move the block down faster. Participants practiced the controls outside of the scanner, using corresponding fingers on a laptop until they were confident about the layout of the controls and how to play Tetris with active mental rotation.

The scanner procedure was 7 min long. It consisted of four cycles of Tetris gameplay (80 s, 60 s, 60 s, 60 s) interleaved with four cycles of motor activity (20 s) directly followed by a resting period (20 s; see Supplementary Fig. S1). In the motor activity condition, participants were instructed to keep pressing the buttons of the hand controls as if still playing Tetris. The motor activity condition always followed Tetris gameplay to keep button pressing as similar as possible between these conditions (participants kept pressing buttons as during immediate preceding gameplay). Instructions during the rest condition (baseline) were to relax and wait for the game to start again. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes open during all conditions (Tetris gameplay, motor activity, rest).

Brain Imaging

Data were acquired using a 3 T whole-body MR scanner (Philips Achieva 3.0 T TX, Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) with a 32-channel head coil. First, an anatomical T1-weighted reference image (voxel size 0.938 × 0.938 × 1; matrix 256 × 256; 220 slices) was collected. During the experimental session, blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) imaging was performed using a single-shot EPI sequence with whole-brain coverage (TE/TR = 35/3000 ms; flip angle 90°; acquired voxel size 1.5 × 1.5 × 3.0, matrix 160 × 160, 45 slices with no gap in ascending order). BOLD images were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space, slice time corrected, and spatially smoothed using an 8 mm FWHM kernel (voxel size 2 × 2 × 2). Preprocessing and all analyses of fMRI-data were performed using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College, London (http://www.fil. ion.ucl.ac.uk).

Data Analysis

For first-level analyses, two experimental regressors were created, one for Tetris gameplay and one for motor activity (i.e., button presses). Rest periods were left to form the baseline. In line with the aim of the study, the primary second-level analysis was the contrast Tetris gameplay relative to motor activity, which assessed brain activations during Tetris gameplay while controlling for motor activity. Additionally, a second-level analysis was performed for Tetris gameplay relative to baseline (rest) to reveal brain activations during Tetris gameplay with accompanying motor activity. Finally, a second-level analysis was conducted for motor activity relative to baseline to show brain activation during motor activity only. Analyses used familywise error correction pFWE < 0.05 over the whole brain. The Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) anatomy toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005) was used to extract activated clusters and label areas of local maximum. The xjView toolbox (https://www.alivelearn.net/xjview) for SPM and the automated anatomical labeling (aal) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) was used to identify and label brain areas located within each cluster.

Results

Results demonstrate that Tetris gameplay, after being given instructions to engage in mental rotation, and when controlling for the motor activity of pressing buttons, activated brain areas in occipital, temporal, parietal, and premotor areas. The global peaks were situated bilaterally in the posterior parts of the inferior temporal gyrus (inferotemporal cortex) and the middle temporal gyrus. Local maxima were found bilaterally in the fusiform gyrus, middle and inferior occipital gyrus, thalamus, superior frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, cerebellum, and the midcingulate cortex. Lateral local maxima were found in several areas, among them the hippocampus. Complete results are presented in Table 1 and further illustrated in Fig. 1. See Supplementary Table S1 for local maxima for both Tetris gameplay relative to motor activity, and motor activity relative to Tetris gameplay, and Supplementary Table S2 for a complete list of brain areas laying within each cluster. Brain activations during Tetris gameplay relative to baseline are illustrated in Fig. 2, reflecting brain activations associated with both the visuospatial (e.g., mental rotation) and motor aspects (button presses to move the blocks) of Tetris gameplay. See Supplementary Table S3 for results regarding Tetris gameplay relative to baseline and Supplementary Table S4 for a complete list of brain areas within each cluster for this contrast. Corresponding results for the contrast motor activity relative to baseline are presented in Supplementary Table S5 (areas of local maximum) and Supplementary Table S6 (brain areas within each cluster). The mean frequency of button presses (i.e button presses per second) during Tetris gameplay (M = 1.76, SD = 0.85) and the motor activity condition (i.e., button presses only; M = 1.58, SD = 0.74) did not differ significantly, t(27) = 1.44, p = 0.16, d = 0.44.

Table 1. Brain areas activated during Tetris gameplay*.
Fig. 1
figure 1

(A) Tetris gameplay after receiving mental rotation instructions, and controlling for motor activity (button presses), recruits brain areas involved in visuospatial processing, including areas in the occipital, temporal, and frontal lobes (premotor areas; see also Table 1). (B) Composite image depicts brain activation of Tetris gameplay, controlling for motor activity (in red: same contrast as in A) in relation to brain activations elicited by motor activity only, showing both contrasts overlaid on the same brain template (axial, sagittal, and coronal view). All images display whole-brain familywise error-corrected results, pFWE < 0.05

Fig. 2
figure 2

Brain activations during Tetris gameplay together with motor activity of pressing buttons. The figure displays the contrast of Tetris gameplay (with button presses) relative to baseline (rest), illustrating both the visuospatial (e.g., mental rotation) and motor aspects (button presses) of Tetris gameplay together

Discussion

The present study aimed to characterize brain areas specifically associated with the visuospatial aspects of Tetris gameplay, by controlling for motor activity (button presses), and using instructions emphasizing mental rotation during gameplay. Results demonstrated that visuospatial processing during Tetris gameplay was associated with robust activation of brain areas across the ventral and dorsal visual stream, i.e., brain areas previously tied to processing of visuospatial information (Shetht & Young, 2016). For instance, the global peak activations during Tetris gameplay were found bilaterally in the posterior parts of the inferior temporal gyrus (inferotemporal cortex) and the middle temporal gyrus. The inferotemporal cortex is at the apex of the ventral visual stream, providing view-invariant identification of objects for use in higher cortical functions (Conway, 2018). This function is consistent with what is performed during the experimental task, where blocks on the screen must be transformed into view-invariant representations for which internal, i.e., mental rotations can be performed. Object recognition has also been related to activity in the neighboring fusiform gyrus (Weiner et al., 2018), which was robustly activated in the current study. Tetris gameplay was also associated with increased activity in premotor areas, even when controlling for motor activity, in line with an engagement in motor planning and decision-making during Tetris gameplay (Hardwick et al., 2013). These activations, together with widespread activation of the occipital cortex, show that Tetris gameplay activates areas tied to visuospatial processing. Thus, findings are consistent with the hypothesis behind emerging interventions targeting dysfunctional mental imagery, that is, that it may be possible to use Tetris gameplay to tax visuospatial processing resources, i.e. compete with mental imagery.

Current results are consistent with findings from previous neuroimaging studies examining other processes related with Tetris gameplay (e.g., the effects of daily practice, task difficulty of Tetris gameplay). In these studies, Tetris gameplay was also associated with activations in the occipital and parietal lobes (Haier et al., 2009; Price et al., 2013) as well as the premotor areas (Haier et al., 2009). Notably, we did find a local maxima (Supplementary Table S1) and many activated voxels within the hippocampus (Supplementary Table S2), an area in which volume has been observed to increase with daily Tetris gameplay practice (3 months; Butler et al., 2020). In the current study, the global peak during Tetris gameplay, controlling for motor activity, was in the middle temporal gyrus (Table 1). Interestingly, experimental studies examining the neural mechanisms involved in the formation of intrusive memories suggest that activation of the middle temporal gyrus may distinguish whether a specific stimulus will become an intrusive memory or not (Bourne et al., 2013; Clark et al., 2016), albeit that the activation in middle temporal gyrus in the present study was further back (BA 19). Thus, this brain area may offer a potential link between visuospatial processing and the formation of memories that become intrusive visual imagery. Future studies should extend our findings by investigating the brain mechanisms underlying Tetris gameplay when it is used as an interference task to target mental imagery-based intrusive memories, i.e., during trauma recall. Relating the reduction in frequency of intrusive memories to neural processing during Tetris gameplay could help uncover underlying mechanisms of how Tetris gameplay can prevent the formation and expression of intrusive memories.

The current study focused on a subtraction design to isolate the neural basis of the visuospatial aspects of Tetris (rather than the motor aspect of button pressing during gameplay), because visuospatial (i.e. imagery-competing) processing has been hypothesized to be one of several important components for the clinical effects of Tetris gameplay when used as an interference task in psychological interventions (Lau-Zhu et al., 2017; Skorka-Brown et al., 2014, 2015). However, the motor aspects of Tetris gameplay (that is, the act of pressing buttons itself) may contribute to the beneficial effects, for instance, by increasing the load on working memory. Indeed, the combination of both visuospatial and motor aspects may be required for Tetris to be effective as an interference task. Furthermore, mental imagery is not restricted to sensory experiences, but can also involve simulating motion and motor behavior. Just as producing mental imagery of an object, and seeing the same object (e.g., a spider), recruit similar neurocircuitry and have a similar impact on emotion (Hoppe et al., 2021; Pearson, 2019), so does imagining and executing a motor action (e.g., running; Munzert et al., 2009). It has been suggested that interference tasks need to be in the same modality as the targeted imagery to be effective (e.g., visuospatial task to target visual mental imagery; Baddeley & Andrade, 2000). Hence, the motor aspects of Tetris could potentially promote the impact on motor imagery (e.g., running away from a perpetrator). Lang has long proposed that mental imagery involves preparedness for action (Lang, 1979), and planning actions and making movements are both components of Tetris gameplay. Future studies should explore the intricate relation between visuospatial and motor aspects of Tetris gameplay and their impact on emotional mental imagery.

Limitations

Some limitations should be taken into consideration when interpreting the current results. The experimental procedure included longer durations of Tetris gameplay (80–60 s/block) compared to the condition of assessing motor activity only (button presses: 20 s/block). Thus, less data (i.e., a smaller number of brain volumes) was collected for motor activity as compared with Tetris gameplay. However, an additional analysis, matching the durations of Tetris gameplay and motor activity to 20 s (i.e., the last 20 s of each period of Tetris gameplay was used in the analysis), produced similar results (see Supplementary Table S7 for a list of local maxima and Supplementary Table S8 for a complete list of brain areas laying within each cluster).

It should also be noted that we could not assess to what extent participants engaged in mental rotation during gameplay, despite instructions to do so. Future studies should explore to what extent mental rotation instructions impact activation of visuospatial processing brain areas, as well as impact associated clinically relevant effects (e.g., the reduction in frequency of intrusive memories). Lastly, the majority of the sample consisted of female participants. Future studies should include a more balanced sample to detect potential gender differences in neural activity associated with visuospatial processing during Tetris gameplay.

Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to characterize brain activity specifically associated with visuospatial processing during Tetris gameplay. Results demonstrate that Tetris gameplay, when controlling for the motor activity of pressing buttons, and after being given instructions to engage in mental rotation, recruits an extensive brain circuitry previously associated with neural processing of visuospatial information. Findings are consistent with the use of Tetris as an imagery-competing task as one of several components of emerging interventions targeting dysfunctional mental imagery. Understanding the underlying neural processes of Tetris gameplay may help to optimize the effectiveness of such interventions, promote the development of novel interventions, as well as contribute to a mechanistic understanding of visuospatial interference tasks.