Skip to main content
Log in

Perceived inequity predicts prejudice towards age-gap relationships

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Age-gap couples often elicit negative stereotypes and prejudice. According to social exchange and equity theories, we predicted that prejudice towards age-gap couples may stem from perceived relational inequity. We hypothesized that age-gap, as compared to age-matched, couples were perceived as less equitable and, as a result, less liked. To test these hypotheses, people evaluated, and inferred the equity of, age-gap and age-matched relationships. We found that age-gap, as compared to age-matched, couples were more disliked and perceived as less equitable. Within age-gap relationships, older men and women were perceived as reaping greater rewards than their younger partners. Importantly, perceived inequity predicted prejudice towards age-gap, but not age-matched, couples. In exploratory analyses, age-gap couples consistently elicited significantly more prejudice than other types of couples. Implications for age-gap relationships and future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is important to note that participants’ prejudice was directed towards the couple, rather than the man or woman specifically. Therefore, the interaction results listed previously could also be written with a focus on women (e.g., “people expressed greater prejudice towards older women’s relationships when they partner with younger, rather than older, men.”)

  2. This interaction could also be written with a focus on women (e.g., “people perceived greater inequity within younger, rather than older, women’s relationships when they partnered with an older man. Such that younger women were perceived to benefit less than older women when they partnered with an older man.”)

  3. For this direct test of our hypothesis, we needed to create a combined perceived inequity for age-gap couples (i.e., older man/younger woman, younger man/older woman) and age-matched couples (i.e., young man/young woman, old man/old woman), respectively. Because the inequity variable was scored from 1 (He’s getting a better deal) to 7 (She’s getting a better deal), we could not simply average these variables together because any perceived inequity among age-gap couples (older man/younger woman, younger man/older woman) would not be captured by an average score. That is, if someone perceived a great deal of inequity among the age-gap older man relationship (score of 1 - He’s getting a much better deal) and age-gap younger man relationship (score of 7 – She’s getting a much better deal), the average (7 + 1)/2 would incorrectly reveal 4 (They’re each getting the same equal deal). Therefore, we needed to take the absolute value of each score from the midpoint of 4 before averaging the scores.

References

  • Adams, J. S. (1963). Toward an understanding of inequity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 422–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, C. A., & Arnold, P. (2001). Opinions towards sexual partners with a large age difference. Marriage and Family Review, 33, 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berscheid, E., Dion, K., Walster, E., & Walster, G. W. (1971). Physical attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the matching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7(2), 173–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(71)90065-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryne, D., & Nelson, D. (1964). Attraction as a function of attitude similarity-dissimilarity: The effect of topic importance. Psychonomic Science, 1(5), 93–94. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03342806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5.

  • Collisson, B., Howell, J. L., Rusbasan, D., & Rosenfeld, E. (2016). Date someone your own size. Prejudice and discrimination towards mixed-weight relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. First published online April, 27, 2016–2540. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407516644067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crow, J. F., & Felsenstein, J. (1968). The effect of assortative mating on the genetic composition of a population. Eugenics Quarterly, 15, 85–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edlund, J. E., & Sagarin, B. J. (2010). Mate value and mate preferences: An investigation into decisions made with and without constraints. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 835–839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 75–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick, D. T., Gabrielidis, C., Keefe, R. C., & Cornelius, J. S. (1996). Adolescents’ age preferences for dating partners: Support for an evolutionary model of life-history strategies. Child Development, 67, 1499–1511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B., Jr., Bahník, Š., Bernstein, M. J., et al. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability: A “many labs” replication project. Social Psychology, 45(3), 142–152. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000178.

  • Lehmiller, J. J., & Agnew, C. R. (2006). Marginalized relationships: The impact of social disapproval on romantic relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 40–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205278710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmiller, J. J., & Agnew, C. R. (2008). Commitment in age-gap heterosexual romantic relationships: A test of evolutionary and socio-cultural predictions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 74–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmiller, J. J., & Agnew, C. R. (2011). May-December paradoxes: An exploration of age-gap relationships in western society. In W. R. Cupach, B. H. Spitzberg, W. R. Cupach, B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.), The dark side of close relationships II (pp. 39–61). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

  • Markey, C., & Markey, P. (2011). Romantic partners, weight status, and weight concerns an examination using the actor-partner interdependence model. Journal of Health Psychology, 16, 217–225. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105310375636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. C., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004a). Perceived reactions to interracial romantic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. C., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004b). Perceived reactions to interracial romantic relationships: When race is used as a cue to status. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7, 354–369. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430204046143.

  • Nakonezny, P. A., & Denton, W. H. (2008). Marital relationships: A social exchange theory perspective. American Journal of Family Therapy, 36(5), 402–412. https://doi.org/10.1080/01926180701647264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (1999). America’s families and living arrangements. Retrieved April 8, 2009 from: http://www.census.gov/population/www/socdemo/hh-fam/p20-537_99.html

  • United Nations. (2000). World Marriage Patterns 2000. Retrieved April 17, 2009 from: http://www.un.org/esa/population/publications/worldmarriage/worldmarriage.htm

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge and greatly appreciate the contributions of my research team: Juliana D’Aoust, Camille Brandon, Angele Doakes, Gabriel Lee, Marlene Martinez, and Crystal Saludes.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian Collisson.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

The author has not received research grants to support this research nor has any conflict of interests.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Collisson, B., De Leon, L.P. Perceived inequity predicts prejudice towards age-gap relationships. Curr Psychol 39, 2108–2115 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9895-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9895-6

Keywords

Navigation