Skip to main content
Log in

Tokenism in Psychology: Standing on the Shoulders of Small Boys

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Tokenism is a situation in which a member of a distinctive category is treated differently from other people. This article is about the situation in which Tokens (people perceived as distinctive) are considered experts on something for having the properties of a token (the thing which makes them distinctive). Tokens who differ by appearance or by being born into another culture might be considered experts on cultures grouped into the same racial/cultural category. Tokens who differ by being skilled in number-related mathematics might be considered experts on the mathematization of phenomena. Tokens might say that some result is valid for all people in some racial/cultural category without sufficient evidence, or use number-related mathematics as a mathematization of psychological phenomena without trying to find more abstract mathematizations. This harms psychological research. A possible future genesis of cultural and number tokenisms is discussed, and some suggestions to improve the discourse offered. The effect of tokenism might be diminished if psychologists focus on more proper thinking about psychological phenomena.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Similar experience are reported from other environments. A German scholar of a Korean heritage might be considered a better expert on Korean culture than other Germans having longer experience with Korea just because of his half-Asian appearance (Froese 2010). Minority scholars in the United States may be given a job at university to be used as representatives of their race, or to be forced to research about diversity issues (Kim, Hall, Anderson, and Willingham 2011; Joseph and Hirshfield 2011).

  2. “A majority of social psychologist who currently self-identify as ‘Asian’ (rather than solely Chinese, Japanese, Pilipino, or Indian etc.) have the common shared intellectual experience of receiving their advanced social psychology training in Western or Westernized university departments.“(Liu and Ng 2007, p. 4)

  3. By the term mathematization I mean mathematical representation of objects.

  4. Furthermore, as shown by Radder (2001), current physics doesn’t contain any universal ontology which might be borrowed by psychology.

  5. Elster (2012) thinks that the utilization of statistical models in the economy is responsible for the bad decisions which led to current economy crisis.

  6. The research practice, where only external induction (empirical methods for collecting and analyzing data) is discussed and internal induction is omitted, is a direct cause of tokenism’s possible survival in psychology. When the discussion of why a psychologist chooses a certain representation of psychological phenomena, and why s/he groups subjects the way s/he does is not part of the standard research process in psychology, it is easy to defend a tokenist position.

  7. Scientific discovery based on introspection might be close to the intuitive scientific style of mathematician Henri Poincaré as discussed by Miller (1997). Poincaré never used notes, never had any plan or goal in mind, nor any idea if the problem is solvable when conducting research. He began writing his papers without knowing what his conclusion would be. For Poincaré, it was necessary to do science as something more than pure logic or evidence of the senses, because with increasing abstraction “the senses would soon become powerless” (p. 56). This something more he called intuition.

  8. The situation in this regard is improving, at least in the United States, since doctoral students’ training in statistics and numerical approaches is deteriorating there (Aiken, West, and Millsap, 2008). American psychologists are therefore thinking more and following algorithms less.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., Millsap, R. E., & Millsap, R. E. (2008). Doctoral Training in Statistics, Measurement, and Methodology in Psychology. Replication and Extension of Aiken, West, Sechrest, and Reno’s (1990) Survey of PhD Programs in North America. American Psychologist, 63, 32–50. doi:10.1037/0003-066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allik, J., Massoudi, K., Realo, A., & Rossier, J. (2012). Personality and Culture. Cross-Cultural Psychology at the Next Crossroads. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 71, 5–12. doi:10.1024/1421-0185/a000069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allwood, C. M. (2012). The Distinction Between Qualitative and Quantitative Research Methods is Problematic. Quality and Quantity, 46, 1417–1429. doi:10.1007/s11135-011-9455-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arendt, H. (1995). Eichmann v Jeruzalémě: Zpráva o Banalitě zla [Eichmann in Jerusalem: A Report on the Banality of Evil]. Mladá Fronta: Praha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%. Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologis, 63, 602–614. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aubin, D. (1997). The Withering Immortality of Nicolas Bourbaki: A Cultural Connector at the Confluence of Mathematics, Structuralism, and the Oulipo in France. Science in Context, 10(2), 297–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aubin, D. (2001). From Catastrophe to Chaos: The Modeling Practices of Applied Topologists. In U. Bottazzini & A. D. Dalmedico (Eds.), Changing Images in Mathematics: From the French Revolution to the new Millenium (pp. 255–279). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bačová, V. (2003). Ciele, Kritériá a Kvalita Výskumu v Psychológii [Aims, Criteria and Quality of Research in Psychology]. Československá psychologie, 47(3), 259–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-Conflict Theory: Progress and Problems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19(1), 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2008). Changing psychologies in the transition from industrial society to consumer society. History of the Human Sciences, 21, 85–110. doi:10.1177/0952695108091412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. R. (1997). What is Applied Mathematics? Foundations of Science, 2(1), 21–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartier, P. (2008). Mathematical Universalism. Diogenes, 219, 67–76. doi:10.1177/0392192108092626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clegg, J. W. (2010). Uncertainty as a Fundamental Scientific Value. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44, 245–251. doi:10.1007/s12124-010-9135-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dobroczyński, B. (2013). Mieczysława Kreutza Obrona Introspekcji: „Łabędzi Śpiew” czy Obietnica Lepszej Przyszłości? [Mieczysław Kreutz’s Defense of Introspection: A „Swan Song“or a Promise for its Better Future?]. Przegląd Psychologiczny, 56(2), 155–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draguns, J. G. (2001). Toward a Truly International Psychology: Beyond English Only. American Psychologist, 56, 1019–1030. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. (2012). Hard and Soft Obscurantism in the Humanities and Social Sciences. Diogenes, 58, 159–170. doi:10.1177/0392192112444984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Froese, F. J. (2010). Acculturation Experiences in Korea and Japan. Culture and Psychology, 16, 333–348. doi:10.1177/1354067X10371138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heller, M. (1997). Essential Tension: Mathematics – Physics – Philosophy. Foundations of Science, 2(1), 39–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, G. J. (2006). An Essay on Inequalities and Order-Restricted Inference. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 50, 215–219. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2006.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, T. D., & Hirshfield, L. E. (2011). ‘Why don’t you get Somebody new to do it?’ Race and Cultural Taxation in Academy. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 34, 121–141. doi:10.1080/01419870.2010.496489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, A. (1964). The Conduct of Inquiry: Methodology for Behavioral Science. San Francisco: Chandler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, C. L., Hall, M. E. L., Anderson, T. L., & Willingham, M. M. (2011). Coping With Discrimination in Academia: Asian-American and Christian Perspectives. Asian American. Journal of Psychology, 2, 291–305. doi:10.1037/a0025552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. London, UK: Lund Humphries. Retrieved from http://www.google.cz/books?hl=cs&lr=&id=yUVGAQAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PT11&dq=Koffka&ots=v5WO5LFgi2&sig=lOrj7E-xonWpzEg46np2zkgeeBg&redir_esc=y#v=onepage&q=Koffka&f=false.

  • Lewin, K. (1930). The Conflict Between Aristotelian and Galileian Modes of Thought in Contemporary Psychology. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 141–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linkov, V. (2013). Comparative Psychological Research in East Asia: An Opportunity for East Asian Studies Scholars. Vienna Journal of East Asian Studies, 4(1), 51–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, J. H., & Ng, S. H. (2007). Connecting Asians in Global Perspective: Special Issue on Past Contributions, Current Status and Future Prospects for Asian Social Psychology. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 1–7. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2006.00204.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marček, V., & Urbánek, T. (2011). Filozoficko-Vedný Koncept Paradigmy v Psychológii [Philosophical and Scientific Concept of Paradigm in Psychology]. Československá psychologie, 55(3), 222–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matoušek, A. (2011). Matematizace a Ovládnutí Přírody [Mathematization and Controlling of the Nature]. In Spor o Matematizaci Světa [Dispute About Mathematization of the World] (pp. 287–295). Červený Kostelec: Pavel Mervart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D., Bennis, W., & Chandler, M. (2010). Culture and the Home-Field Disadvantage. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 708–713. doi:10.1177/1745691610388772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2000). Normal Science, Pathological Science and Psychometrics. Theory and Psychology, 10, 639–667. doi:10.1177/0959354300105004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2003). The Quantitative Imperative: Positivism, Naive Realism and the Place of Qualitative Methods in Psychology. Theory and Psychology, 13, 5–31. doi:10.1177/0959354303013001758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2004). The Place of Qualitative Research in Psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 1, 307–319. doi:10.1191/1478088704qp020oa.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2005). The Meaning of Qunatitative Imperative. A response to Niaz. Theory and Psychology, 15, 257–263. doi:10.1177/0959354305051369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2011). Qualitative Research Meets the Ghost of Pythagoras. Theory and Psychology, 21, 241–259. doi:10.1177/0959354310391351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. I. (1997). Cultures of Creativity. Mathematics and Physics. Diogenes, 45(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mok, T. A. (1998). Getting the Message: Media Images and Stereotypes and Their Effect on Asian Americans. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 4, 185–202. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.4.3.185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niemann, Y. F. (2003). The Psychology of Tokenism. Psychosocial Realities of Faculty of Color. In G. Bernal, J. E. Trimble, A. K. Burlew, & F. T. L. Leong (Eds.), Handbook of Racia (Ethnic Minority Psychology, pp. 100–118). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obradovic, N., & Ninkovic, N. (2009). The Heuristic Function of Mathematics in Physics and Astronomy. Foundations of Science, 14, 351–360. doi:10.1007/s10699-009-9162-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, T. M. (1995). Trust in Numbers. The Pursuit of Objectivity in Science and Public Life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pourciau, B. (2000). Intuitionism as a (Failed) Kuhnian Revolution in Mathematics. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 31, 297–329. doi:10.1016/S0039-3681(00)00010-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radder, H. (2001). Psychology, Physicalism and Real Physics. Theory and Psychology, 11, 773–784. doi:10.1177/0959354301116004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodgers, J. L. (2010). The Epistemology of Mathematical and Statistical Modeling. A Quiet Methodological Revolution. American Psychologist, 65, 1–12. doi:10.1037/a0018326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodych, V. (2000). Wittgenstein’s Critique of set Theory. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 38, 281–319. doi:10.1111/j.2041-6962.2000.tb00902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roehrig, T. (2010). History as a Strategic Weapon: The Korean and Chinese Struggle Over Koguryo. Journal of Asian and African Studies, 45, 5–28. doi:10.1177/0021909610352675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, L. (Ed.). (2013). Qualitative Mathematics for the Social Sciences. Mathematical Models for Research on Cultural Dynamics. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, N. (2001). The Limits of Human Mathematics. Philosophical perspectives, 15, 93–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stöltzner, M. (2004). On Optimism and Opportunism in Applied Mathematics: Mark Wilson Meets John von Neumann on Mathematical Ontology. Erkenntnis, 60(1), 121–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L. (2013). Generalization as Creative and Reflective act: Revisiting Lewin’s Conflict Between Aristotelian and Galilean Modes of Thought in Psychology. Theory and Psychology, 23, 518–536. doi:10.1177/0959354313488844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thissen, D. (2001). Psychometric Engineering as art. Psychometrika, 66, 473–486. doi:10.1007/BF02296190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trlifajová, K. (2005). Teologické Zdůvodnění Cantorovy Teorie Množin [A Theological Substantiation of Cantor’s set Theory]. Filosofický časopis, 53(2), 195–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2006). Dangerous Curves in Knowledge Construction Within Psychology. Fragmentation of methodology. Theory and Psychology, 16, 597–612. doi:10.1177/0959354306067439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vybíral, Z. (2006). Psychologie Jinak. Současná Kritická Psychologie [Psychology Being Done Other way. Contemporary Critical Psychology]. Praha: Academia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigner, E. P. (1960). The Unreasonable Effectiveness of Mathematics in the Natural Sciences. Communication on pure and applied mathematics, 13, 1–14. doi:10.1002/cpa.3160130102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2000). The Unreasonable Uncooperativeness of Mathematics in the Natural Sciences. The Monist, 83(2), 296–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I was a recipient of Taiwan scholarship received from MOE of Taiwan (R.O.C.) when I began writing this text. I thank Vladimír Marček, Jaan Valsiner, Todd Hammond and anonymous reviewers for advices on how to improve this text, and Kao Yuang-Kuang (高永光) for introducing me to Kaplan’s book. I also thank Lubomír Kostroň for inspiration.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Václav Linkov.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Linkov, V. Tokenism in Psychology: Standing on the Shoulders of Small Boys. Integr. psych. behav. 48, 143–160 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-014-9266-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-014-9266-2

Keywords

Navigation