Skip to main content
Log in

Imagination as Expansion of Experience

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper proposes a developmental view on imagination: from this perspective, imagination can be seen as triggered by some disrupting event, which generates a disjunction from the person’s unfolding experience of the “real” world, and as unfolding as a loop, which eventually comes back to the actual experience. Examining recent and classical theorization of imagination in psychology, the paper opposes a deficitary view of imagination to an expansive notion of imagination. The paper explores Piaget, Vygotsky, Harris and Pelaprat & Cole consider: 1) What does provoke a “rupture” or disjunction? 2) What are the psychological processes involved in the imaginary loop? 3) What nourishes such processes? 4) What are the consequences of such imaginary loop, or what does it enable doing? The paper proposes to adopt an expansive view of imagination, as Vygotsky proposed—a perspective that has been under-explored empirically since his seminal work. To stimulate such sociocultural psychology of imagination, two empirical examples are provided, one showing how children make sense of metaphor in an experimental setting, the other showing a young person using a novel met at school as symbolic resource.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Plato (427–346 bc) considered imagination as the lowest degree of knowledge. In the 16 and 16th centuries, authors mostly mistrust its dangers: imagination is seen as “the mad in the house [la folle du logis] (…) fertile source of aberration and illusion” (Malebranche 1990).

  2. Kant saw imagination as one middle term between perception and concept, as bridging term between actions and thinking. He proposed to distinguish on the one side “reproductive imagination”, the capacity to represent objects in their absence, thus linking perception and memory, and on the other, “productive or creative imagination”, freely operating on the basis of reality and combining images in a new way. This distinction was then taken on later by many thinkers, in particular by Ribot (1900/2007) in the field of psychology.

  3. « (…) l’évolution ultérieure de l’imagination symbolique consistera précisément en son amoindrissement au profit de moyens de représentation plus adaptés au réel (…) Au fond, l’enfant n’a pas d’imagination, et celle que le sens commun lui attribue se réduit à l’incohérence et surtout à l’assimilation subjective dont témoignent ses transpositions. (p.138) (…) Mais pourquoi existe-t-il une assimilation du réel au moi, au lieu que l’univers soit d’emblée assimilé à la pensée logique et expérimentale ? Tout simplement parce que cette pensée n’est pas encore construite durant la petite enfance. (p. 175) » (Piaget 1994a, b).

  4. « Le syncrétisme est la tendance spontanée des enfants à percevoir par visions globales au lieu de discerner les détails, à trouver des analogies immédiatement, sans analyse, entre des objets et des mots étrangers les uns aux autres, à lier entre eux des phénomènes naturels hétérogènes, à trouver une raison à tout événement même fortuit, bref c’est la tendance à tout lier à tout (…). » (Piaget 1978, p. 9)

  5. « On peut distinguer parmi [les fonctions cognitives] deux grandes catégories (…) de la connaissance : l’aspect figuratif et l’aspect opératif. Le premier tend à atteindre les aspects figuraux de la réalité. L’aspect opératif caractérise au contraire les formes d’expérience cognitive ou de déduction consistant à modifier l’objet de manière à atteindre les transformations comme telles. » (Piaget 1999, p. 22–23)

  6. « Pour une pensée égocentrique, le jeu tient en somme lieu de loi suprême. C’est l’un des mérites de la psychanalyse d’avoir montré que l’autisme ne connaît pas l’adaptation au réel, parce que, pour le moi, le plaisir est le seul ressort. La pensée autistique a ainsi pour unique fonction de donner aux besoins et aux intérêts une satisfaction immédiate et sans contrôle, en déformant le réel pour l’adapter au moi » (Piaget 1978, p.193).

  7. SYRES project (Symbolic resources in secondary school), by T. Zittoun and M. Grossen, and the collaboration of O. Lempen, C. Matthey, S. Padiglia and J. Ros, supported by the Swiss National Fund.

  8. Ricoeur’s hermeneutic analysis goes in the same direction; for him, creativity in the discourse can occur through metaphors which are themselves embedded in language which has the capacity to trigger specific meanings—therefore his notion of “canonicity of sense”; however, his analysis is not sociocultural.

References

  • Archambault, A., & Venet, M. (2007). Le développement de l’imagination selon Piaget et Vygotsky : d’un acte spontané à une activité consciente. Revue des sciences de l’éducation, 33(1), 5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The rational imagination: how people create alternatives to reality. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Cerchia, F. (2009). Young children’s use of symbolic resources in an experimental setting testing metaphor comprehension. Psychology & Society, 2(2), 200–211. Available online at:www.psychologyandsociety.org/__assets/__original/2009/11/cerchia.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cerchia, F. (2011). L’enfant et la métaphore. Percée socio-culturelle dans les contours normatifs du cognitivisme. Phd thesis, University of Lausanne, Switzerland.

  • Collier, M. (1999). Filling the gaps: Hume and connectionism on the continued existence of unperceived objects. Hume Studies, 25(1–2), 155–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crapanzano, V. (2004). Imaginative horizons: An essay in literary-philosophical anthropology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Moraes Ramos de Oliveira, Z., & Valsiner, J. (1997). Play and imagination. The psychological construction of novelty. In A. Fogel, M. C. D. P. Lyra, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Dynamics and indeterminism in developmental and social processes (pp. 119–132). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1996). In J. Cottingham (Ed.), Descartes: Meditations on first philosophy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press [Original publication 1647].

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Díaz, J. L. (2010). Sacred plants and visionary consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 159–170. doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9157-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbers, E., & Kelderman, A. (1994). Ground rules for testing: Expectations and misunderstandings in test situations. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9, 111–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Eluard, P. (1929). L’Amour la poésie. Paris: Gallimard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, F. S. (2000). The great Gatsby. London: Penguin Classics [Original publication 1950].

    Google Scholar 

  • Furlong, E.J. (2004). Imagination. Routledge. [Original publication 1961].

  • Gillespie, A. (2006). Games and the development of perspective taking. Human Development, 49(2), 87–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossen, M., Zittoun, T., & Ros, J. (2012). Boundary crossing events and potential appropriation space in philosophy, literature and general knowledge. In E. Hjörne, G. van der Aalsvoort, & G. de Abreu (Eds.), Learning, social interaction and diversity—exploring school practices (pp. 15–33). Rotterdam/Boston/Taipei: Sense Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (2000). The work of the imagination. Oxford/Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (2007). L’imagination chez l’enfant : son rôle crucial dans le développement cognitif et affectif. Paris: Retz.

  • Hume, D. (1896). In M. A. Selby-Bigge (Ed.), A treatise of human nature. Oxford: Clarendon. [Original Edition in three volumes, original manuscript 1739.]. Available online at http://michaeljohnsonphilosophy.com/wp-content/uploads/2012/01/5010_Hume_Treatise_Human_Nature.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. I). New York: Dover publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago/London: University of Chicago Press/Weidenfeld and Nicolson Ltd [Original French publication 1962].

    Google Scholar 

  • Malebranche, N. (1990). De l’imagination. De la Recherche de la vérité, livre II. Paris: Vrin [Original French publication 1675].

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, S., & John-Steiner, V. (2003). Creativity in the making: Vygotsky’s contemporary attribution to the dialectic of development and creativity. In R. K. Sawyer, V. John-Steiner, S. Moran, R. J. Sternberg, D. H. Feldman, J. Nakamura, & Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Creativity and development (pp. 61–90). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P., Peters, M.A., Marginson, S. (2010). Imagination: three models of imagination in the age of the knowledge economy. Peter Lang.

  • Oatley, K. (2011). Such stuff as dreams: The psychology of fiction. Malden, MA/Oxford: Wiley.

  • Obeyesekere, G. (1990). The work of culture: Symbolic transformation in psychoanalysis and anthropology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1877). The fixation of belief. Popular Science Monthly, 12, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelaprat, E., & Cole, M. (2011). “Minding the gap”: imagination, creativity and human cognition. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 45, 397–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press [Original French publication 1936].

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962a). (with B. Inhelder). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Book.

  • Piaget, J. (1962b). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. [Original French publication 1945]

  • Piaget, J. (1969). Judgement and reasoning in the child. Totowa: Littlefield Adams [Original French publication 1924].

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1978). Le jugement et le raisonnement chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé [Original publication 1924].

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1994a). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestlé [Original publication 1936].

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1994b). La formation du symbole chez l’enfant: Imitation, jeu et rêve, image et représentation (8e éd.). Delachaux & Niestle. [Original publication 1945]

  • Piaget, J. (1999). (with B. Inhelder). La psychologie de l’enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. [Original French publication 1966].

  • Ribot, T. (2007). Essai sur l’imagination créatrice. Paris: L’Harmattan (1st ed. 1900).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricoeur, P. (1975). La métaphore vive. Paris: Seuil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricoeur, P. (1978). Th metaphorical process as cognition, imagination, and feeling. Critical Inquiry, 5(1, Special Issue on Metaphor), 143–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, I. (Ed.). (2007). Imaginative minds: Concepts, controversies and themes (1st ed.). Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press/British Academy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, L., & Livesay, H. (2006). Look, up in the sky! Using superheroes in play therapy. International Journal of Play Therapy, 15(1), 117–133. doi:10.1037/h0088911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. (1992). The house of make-believe: children’s play and the developing imagination. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. (2005). Imagination and play in the electronic age. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolucha, L., & Smolucha, F.C. (1986). L. S. Vygotsky’s theory of creative imagination. Paper presented as the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association (94th), Washington, DC.

  • Valsiner, J. (2003). Beyond social representations: a theory of enablement. Papers on Social Representations, 12, 7.1–7.16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.-S. (2011). L’imagination et son développement chez l’enfant. In Leçons de psychologie (p. 155–180). Paris: La Dispute.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). The psychology of art. Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (2002). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. [Original publication 1933]. Retrieved July 22, 2010, from http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1933/play.htm.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (2004). Imagination and creativity in childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42(1), 7–97 [Original publication 1930 in Soviet Psychology 28 (1): 84–96].

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B., Gillespie, A., Valsiner, J., Zittoun, T., Salgado, J., & Simao, L. M. (2011). Repairing ruptures: Multivocality of analyses. In M. Märtsin, B. Wagoner, E.-L. Aveling, I. Kadianiki, & L. Whittaker (Eds.), Dialogicality in focus: Challenges to theory, method and application (pp. 105–127). Hauppauge: Nova.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott, D. W. (2001). Playing and reality. Philadelphia/Sussex: Routledge. [Original publication 1971].

  • Zittoun, T. (2004). Symbolic competencies for developmental transitions: the case of the choice of first names. Culture & Psychology, 10(2), 131–161. doi:10.1177/1354067X04040926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2006a). Transitions. Development through symbolic resources. Greenwich (CT): InfoAge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2006b). Difficult secularity: Talmud as symbolic resource. Outlines. Critical Social Studies, 8(2), 59–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2007a). Symbolic resources and responsibility in transitions. Young. Nordic Journal of Youth Research, 15(2), 193–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2007b). Processes of interiority. In L. Simão & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Otherness in question: Development of the self (pp. 187–214). Greenwich (CT): InfoAge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2008). Sign the gap: dialogical self in disrupted times. Studia Psychologica, 6(8), 73–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2013). As if for the first time: Cultural experiences as symbolic resources. In A. Kuhn (Ed.), Little madnesses: Winnicott, transitional phenomena and cultural experience (pp. 135–147). London: Tauris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T., & Grossen, M. (2012). Cultural elements as means of constructing the continuity of the self across various spheres of experience. In M. César & B. Ligorio (Eds.), The interplays between dialogical learning and dialogical self (pp. 99–126). Charlotte: InfoAge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T., Valsiner, J., Vedeler, D., Salgado, J., Gonçalves, M., & Ferring, D. (2013). Human development in the lifecourse. Melodies of living. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank three anonymous reviewers for their careful and useful work, as well as their students’ questions, which allowed improving a first version of this text.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tania Zittoun.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zittoun, T., Cerchia, F. Imagination as Expansion of Experience. Integr. psych. behav. 47, 305–324 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-013-9234-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-013-9234-2

Keywords

Navigation