Skip to main content
Log in

How Language Enables Abstraction: A Study in Computational Cultural Psychology

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The idea that language mediates our thoughts and enables abstract cognition has been a key idea in socio-cultural psychology. However, it is not clear what mechanisms support this process of abstraction. Peirce argued that one mechanism by which language enables abstract thought is hypostatic abstraction, the process through which a predicate (e.g., dark) turns into an object (e.g., darkness). By using novel computational tools we tested Peirce’s idea. Analysis of the data provides empirical support for Peirce’s mechanism and evidence of the way the use of signs enables abstraction. These conclusions are supported by the in-depth analysis of two case studies concerning the abstraction of sweet and dark. The paper concludes by discussing the findings from a broad and integrative theoretical perspective and by pointing to computational cultural psychology as a promising perspective for addressing long-lasting questions of the field.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The corpus was collected by Charles Clarke at the University of Waterloo.

  2. Wumpus is available at http://www.wumpus-search.org/.

  3. WordNet is available at http://wordnet.princeton.edu/.

  4. The dictionary is available at http://ota.oucs.ox.ac.uk/headers/1054.xml.

  5. A copy of the 114,501 rated terms is available on request from Peter Turney.

References

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Continuity of the conceptual system across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 309–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Büttcher, S., & Clarke, C. (2005). Efficiency vs. effectiveness in terabyte-scale information retrieval. Proceedings of the 14th Text REtrieval Conference (TREC 2005), Gaithersburg, MD.

  • Caron, J. (2001). Experiments with LSA scoring: Optimal rank and basis. Proceedings of the SIAM Computational Information Retrieval Workshop (pp. 157–169). Raleigh, NC.

  • Church, K. W., & Hanks, P. (1989). Word association norms, mutual information and lexicography. Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Association of Computational Linguistics (pp. 76–83). Association for Computational Linguistics, New Brunswick, NJ.

  • Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 370–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce [CP] (Vols. 1–8). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (Vols. 1–6, C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss, Eds.; Vols. 7–8, A. W. Burks, Ed.).

  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A(4), 497–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danesi, M. (2003). Metaphorical “networks” and verbal communication: a semiotic perspective on human discourse. Sign Systems Studies, 31, 341–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, M. (2008). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA): 410+ million words, 1990-present. Retrieved from http://www.americancorpus.org.

  • de Saussure, F. (1973). Course in General Linguistics (R. Harris, Trans.). London: Duckworth.

  • Deutscher, G. (2010). Does your language shape how you think? The New York Times, 29 August 2010.

  • Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of the acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, J. A., & Valsiner, J. (1993). Conceptual roots of internalization: from transmission to transformation. Human Development, 36, 150–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P. (1996). The Whorf theory complex. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, R. A., & Vygotsky, L. S. (1930). Ape, primitive man and child. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., Aiden, E. L. (2010). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science [10.1126/science.1199644].

  • Neuman, Y. (2003). Processes and boundaries of the mind: Extending the limit line. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y. (2008). Reviving the living: Meaning making in living systems. Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y. (2009). On love, hate and knowledge. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 90, 697–712.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y., & Nave, O. (2009). Metaphor-based meaning excavation. Information Sciences, 179, 2719–2728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, Y., & Nave, O. (2010). Why the brain needs language in order to be self-conscious. New Ideas in Psychology, 28, 37–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Francis, M. E. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count. USA: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., & Castelfranchi, C. (2007). The symbol detachment problem. Cognitive Processes, 8, 115–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, R. (2003). Word sense discovery based on sense descriptor dissimilarity. Proceedings of the Ninth Machine Translation Summit (pp. 315–322). New Orleans.

  • Salvatore, S., & Venuleo, C. (2008). Understanding the role of emotion in sense making: a semiotic psychoanalytic oriented perspective. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42(1), 32–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis method. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29, 24–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tobin, Y. (1990). Semiotics and linguistics. London: Longman Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turney, P. D., & Littman, M. L. (2003). Measuring praise and criticism: Inference of semantic orientation from association. ACM Transactions on Information Systems (TOIS), 21(4), 315–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turney, P. D., & Pantel, P. (2010). From frequency to meaning: vector space models of semantics. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research (JAIR), 37, 141–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in minds and societies. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vauclair, J. (2003). Would humans without language be apes? In J. Valsiner & A. Toomela (Eds.), Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind. Vol 7. Advances in child development within culturally structured environment (pp. 9–26). Greenwich: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volosinov, V. (1986). Marxism and the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Harvard UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Harvard, MA: The MIT Press. Electronic source: Thinking and speaking http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/words/index.htm

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yair Neuman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Neuman, Y., Turney, P. & Cohen, Y. How Language Enables Abstraction: A Study in Computational Cultural Psychology. Integr. psych. behav. 46, 129–145 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-011-9165-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-011-9165-8

Keywords

Navigation