Skip to main content
Log in

Quantitative and Qualitative Research: Beyond the Debate

  • Commentary
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 16 September 2009

Abstract

Psychology has been a highly quantitative field since its conception as a science. However, a qualitative approach to psychological research has gained increasing importance in the last decades, and an enduring debate between quantitative and qualitative approaches has arisen. The recently developed Mixed Methods Research (MMR) addresses this debate by aiming to integrate quantitative and qualitative approaches. This article outlines and discusses quantitative, qualitative and mixed methods research approaches with specific reference to their (1) philosophical foundations (i.e. basic sets of beliefs that ground inquiry), (2) methodological assumptions (i.e. principles and formal conditions which guide scientific investigation), and (3) research methods (i.e. concrete procedures for data collection, analysis and interpretation). We conclude that MMR may reasonably overcome the limitation of purely quantitative and purely qualitative approaches at each of these levels, providing a fruitful context for a more comprehensive psychological research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Objectivism is often associated with quantitative research approaches and has been articulated at a meta-theoretical and philosophical level in logical positivism and critical rationalism. On the contrary, subjectivism and constructivism are typically associated with qualitative investigation, and have been expressed at a meta-theoretical and philosophical level, among others, in phenomenology, hermeneutics and symbolic interactionism.

  2. According to the degree of exerted experimental control, it is possible to distinguish between true-experiments and quasi-experiments (for a detailed description see Polgar and Thomas 2000).

  3. Non-experimental designs include correlational designs, correlational–comparative designs, correlational–causal–comparative designs, and ex-post-facto designs.

  4. Case study designs may also be appropriate to the quantitative approach. In this case it is usual to talk about single-case research designs (see Hilliard 1993 and Kazdin 1982).

  5. The term focus group may also refer to a specific form of data collection (see next paragraph).

  6. For a detailed description of different typologies of validity as well as of validity threats and strategies for addressing these threats see Campbell and Stanley 1963 and Cook and Campbell 1979.

  7. Generalizability has also been referred to as transferability (Guba and Lincoln 1989).

  8. Whereby the visual data is usually video-recorded in order to allow the subsequent analysis according to the specific observational protocol used.

  9. See e.g. the Analysis and Treatment of Finger Sucking (Ellingson et al. 2000), which allows investigating the reinforcements useful in maintaining finger sucking, or the Strange Situation Protocol (Ainsworth et al. 1978) for the assessment of attachment in infants (12–20 months).

  10. Some forms of open-ended interview (i.e. the interview guide approach and the standardized open-ended interview) correspond to some extent to what is generally known as semi-structured interview.

  11. The roles of an observer may vary on a continuum: complete participant, to the participant-as-observer, observer-as-participant, and complete observer (Johnson and Turner 2003).

  12. This is the case of mixed methods designs, which are described in the present paper. In mixed model designs (for on overview see Tashakkori and Teddlie 2003a), by contrast, interpretation takes place after the application of each quantitative and qualitative strand of the design. The researcher has then to go through a process of meta-interpretation. The inferences developed for each strand of the design are then integrated. This process is called meta-inference (Tashakkori and Teddlie 2003a).

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: a psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism: Perspective and method. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryman, A. (1984). Quantity and quality in social research. London: Unwin Hyman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryman, A. (1988). Quantity and quality in social research. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research on teaching. In N. L. Gage (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 171–246). Chigago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi experimentation: Designs and analysis issues for field settings. Boston: Houghton Miffli.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooksy, L. J., Gill, P., & Kelly, P. A. (2001). The program logic model as an integrative framework for a multimethod evaluation. Evaluation and Program Planning, 24, 119–128. doi:10.1016/S0149-7189(01)00003-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Creswell, J. W. (2005). Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Creswell, J. W., & Plano Clark, V. L. (2007). Designing and conducting. Mixed methods research. Thousand Oaks. CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Creswell, J. W., Plano Clark, V. L., Gutmann, M., & Hanson, W. (2003). Advanced mixed methods research designs. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 209–204). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1985). The methodological imperative in psychology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 15, 1–13. doi:10.1177/004839318501500101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, S. L. (Eds.). (2005). The Sage handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • Dilthey, W. (1989). Introduction to the human sciences: An attempt to lay a foundation for the study of society and history. In R. A. Makkreel, & F. Rodi (Eds.), Selected works. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellingson, S. A., Miltenberger, R. G., Stricker, J. M., Garlinghouse, M. A., Roberts, J., & Rapp, J. T. (2000). Analysis and treatment of finger sucking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 41–52. doi:10.1901/jaba.2000.33-41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, M. S., & Williams, D. I. (2002). A qualitative evaluation of an employee counselling service from the perspective of client, counsellor and organization. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 15(2), 201–208. doi:10.1080/09515070210128991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, L., & Hardy, L. (2002a). Injury rehabilitation: a goal-setting intervention study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73, 310–319.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, L., & Hardy, L. (2002b). Injury rehabilitation: a qualitative follow-up study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73, 320–329.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., Gallimore, R., & Reese, L. (2005). Using mixed methods to explore Latino children’s literacy development. In T. S. Weisner (Ed.), Discovering successful pathways in children’s development: Mixed methods in the study of childhood and family life (pp. 283–303). Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. C. (2000). Challenges in practicing deliberative democratic evaluation. In K. R. Ryan, & L. DeStefano (Eds.), Evaluation as a democratic process: Promoting inclusion, dialogue and deliberation New Directions for Evaluation, No. 85 (pp. 13–26). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. C., & Caracelli, V. J. (2003). Making paradigmatic sense of mixed methods practice. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 91–110). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1989). Fourth generation evaluation. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2005). Paradigm controversies, contradictions, and emerging confluences. In N. K. Denzin, & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research (pp. 191–215, 3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilliard, R. B. (1993). Single-case methodology in psychotherapy process and outcome research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 373–380. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.61.3.373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, K. R. (1988). Against the quantitative–qualitative incompatibility thesis or dogmas die hard. Educational Researcher, 17(8), 10–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. B., & Christensen, L. B. (2000). Educational research: Quantitative and qualitative approaches. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. B., & Turner, L. A. (2003). Data collection strategies in mixed methods research. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook on mixed methods in the behavioral and social sciences (pp. 297–320). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, E., Stringfield, S., & Teddlie, C. (2003). Mixed methods sampling strategies in social science research. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 273–296). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köckeis-Stangl, E. (1980). Interpretative Methoden kontrollierten Fremdverstehens. In K. Hurrelmann, & D. Ulrich (Eds.), Handbuch der Sozialisationsforschung. Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, D. L. (1995). Sustaining versus resolving the quantitative–qualitative debate. Evaluation and Program Planning, 18, 89–96. doi:10.1016/0149-7189(94)00052-Y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamiell, J. T. (1998). Nomothetic and idiographic: Contrasting Windelband’s understanding with contemporary usage. Theory & Psychology, 8(1), 23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. A. (1992). Understanding and validity in qualitative research. Harvard Educational Review, 62(3), 279–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. A., & Loomis, D. M. (2003). Mixed methods design: An alternative approach. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, D. B., & Etkina, E. (2002). College physics students’ epistemological self-reflection and its relationship to conceptual learning. American Journal of Physics, 70(12), 1249–1258. doi:10.1119/1.1503377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (1999). Measurement in psychology: A critical history of a methodological concept. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2000). Normal science, pathological science and psychometrics. Theory & Psychology, 10(5), 639–667. doi:10.1177/0959354300105004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. (1994). Critical rationalism. A restatement and defence. Chicago and La Salle: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, D. (2000). Introduction to phenomenology. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, J. M. (2003). Principles of mixed methods and multimethod research design. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 189–208). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, I., & Benz, C. R. (1998). Qualitative–quantitative research methodology: Exploring the interactive continuum. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, I., Ridenour, C. S., Newman, C., & DeMarco, G. M. P. (2003). A typology of research purpose and its relationship to mixed methods. In N. K. Denzin, & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research (pp. 167–188, 3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noblitt, G. W., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Meta-ethnography: Synthesizing qualitative studies. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Teddlie, C. (2003). A framework for analyzing data in mixed methods research. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 351–383). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagano, M. E., Hirsch, B. J., Deutsch, N. L., & McAdams, D. P. (2002). The transmission of values to school-age and young adult offspring: race and gender differences in parenting. Journal of Feminist Family Therapy, 14, 13–36. doi:10.1300/J086v14n03_02.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patton, M. Q. (1988). Paradigms and pragmatism. In D. M. Fetterman (Ed.), Qualitative approaches to evaluation in education: The silent scientific revolution (pp. 116–137). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polgar, S., & Thomas, S. (2000). Introduction to research in the health sciences (4th ed.). Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Punch, K. F. (1998). Introduction to social research: Quantitative and qualitative approaches. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1990). Realism with a human face. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichardt, C. S., & Cook, T. D. (1979). Beyond qualitative versus quantitative methods. In T. D. Cook, & C. S. Reichardt (Eds.), Qualitative and quantitative methods in evaluation research (pp. 7–32). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, A. (1988). Philosophy of social science. Boulder, CO: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandelowsky, M. (2003). Tables or tableux? The challenges of writing and reading mixed methods studies. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 321–350). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sayer, A. (2000). Realism and social science. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sieber, S. D. (1973). The integration of field work and survey methods. American Journal of Sociology, 78, 1335–1359. doi:10.1086/225467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, D. (2004). Qualitative research: Theory, method and practice (2nd ed.). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. K. (1983). Quantitative versus qualitative research: an attempt to clarify the issue. Educational Researcher, 12(3), 6–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. A. (Ed.). (2003). Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to research methods.(2nd ed). London: Sage.

  • Smith, J. K., & Heshiusius, L. (1986). Closing down the conversation: the end of the quantitative–qualitative debate among educational researchers. Educational Researcher, 15(4), 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steckler, A., McLeroy, K. R., Goodman, R. M., BIrd, S. T., & McCormick, L. (1992). Toward integrating qualitative and quantitative methods: An introduction. Health Education Quarterly, 19(1), 1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (1998). Mixed methodology: Combining qualitative and quantitative approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (2003a). The past and the future of mixed methods research: From data triangulation to mixed model designs. In A. Tashakkori, & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 671–701). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (Eds.). (2003b). Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • Todd, Z., Nerlich, B., & McKeown, (2004). Introduction. In Z. Todd, B. Nerlich, S. McKeown, & D. Clarke (Eds.), Mixing methods in psychology: The integration of qualitative and quantitative methods in theory and practice (pp. 3–16). Hove & New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2008). Variables in psychology: a critique of quantitative psychology. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 42, 3. doi:10.1007/s12124-008-9059-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, D. A., Sweet, D., & Pretty, I. A. (2002). The emotional and psychological impact of mass casualty incidents on forensic odontologists. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 47(3), 539–541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westermann, R. (1987). Wissenschaftstheoretische Grundlagen der experimentellen Psychologie. In G. Lüger (Ed.), Allgemeine experimentelle psychologie (pp. 5–42). Stuttgart: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Omar Gelo.

Additional information

An erratum to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12124-009-9107-x

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gelo, O., Braakmann, D. & Benetka, G. Quantitative and Qualitative Research: Beyond the Debate. Integr. psych. behav. 42, 266–290 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-008-9078-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-008-9078-3

Keywords

Navigation