Skip to main content
Log in

Household and Kin Provisioning by Hadza Men

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We use data collected among Hadza hunter-gatherers between 2005 and 2009 to examine hypotheses about the causes and consequences of men’s foraging and food sharing. We find that Hadza men foraged for a range of food types, including fruit, honey, small animals, and large game. Large game were shared not like common goods, but in ways that significantly advantaged producers’ households. Food sharing and consumption data show that men channeled the foods they produced to their wives, children, and their consanguineal and affinal kin living in other households. On average, single men brought food to camp on 28% of days, married men without children at home on 31% of days, and married men with children at home on 42% of days. Married men brought fruit, the least widely shared resource, to camp significantly more often than single men. A model of the relationship between hunting success and household food consumption indicates that the best hunters provided 3–4 times the amount of food to their families than median or poor hunters. These new data fill important gaps in our knowledge of the subsistence economy of the Hadza and uphold predictions derived from the household and kin provisioning hypotheses. Key evidence and assumptions backing prior claims that Hadza hunting is largely a form of status competition were not replicated in our study. In light of this, family provisioning is a more viable explanation for why good hunters are preferred as husbands and have higher fertility than others.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. On marital classifications: Men who were living with a spouse were classified as married, and otherwise, single. Although some men living alone might claim to be married, from a household provisioning standpoint, having a wife who is not co-resident is essentially equivalent to having no wife.

  2. On kinship: Using a more restrictive definition of kin, considering only individuals related by r > 0.125 to the food producer or his wife, corroborates this evidence for kin-biases in consumption (consanguineal vs. non-kin: χ2 = 106.0, p < 0.0005; affinal vs. non-kin: χ2 = 6.2, p = 0.01).

  3. On focal follows: Hawkes et al. (1991) report data derived from 45 follows conducted in 1985–1986 covering 75 hunter-days (average = 1.6 hunters per follow). In these follows, 14 small game animals were killed.

  4. On Hawkes et al. (2001a) sharing data: Case numbers 22, 23, 24, 30, 35, 37, 42, 44, 46, 47, 49, and 65 as reported in Hawkes et al. (2001a:138). In the remaining cases, some or all of the producer household shares were not reported.

  5. On selecting the max, the median, and the minimum hunters: Selecting the maximum-producing hunter in each camp is straightforward. Selecting the median hunter is straightforward in camps with an odd number of hunters. In camps with an even number, we first removed the best hunter from the sample and then identified the median hunter from among those men who remained. Since the distribution of hunting success among men is usually right-skewed (a few hunters with very high success), this is a better estimate of central tendency. Selecting the minimum hunter was a little tricky in two camps, where more than one man produced zero large game or small game. In these instances, we randomly selected one of the men.

References

  • Allen-Arave, W., Gurven, M., & Hill, K. (2008). Reciprocal altruism, rather than kin selection, maintains nepotistic food transfers on an Ache reservation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 305–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berbesque, J., & Marlowe, F. (2009). Sex differences in food preferences of Hadza hunter-gatherers. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(4), 601–616.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betzig, L., & Turke, P. (1986). Food sharing on Ifaluk. Current Anthropology, 27(4), 397–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. (1978). Nunamiut ethnoarchaeology. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., Bird, D., Smith, E., & Kushnick, G. (2002). Risk and reciprocity in Meriam food sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(4), 297–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (1984). Selfish origin for human food sharing: tolerated theft. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5(1), 1–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (1987). Tolerated theft: suggestions about the ecology and evolution of sharing, hoarding, and scrounging. Social Science Information, 26, 31–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., Smith, L. C., O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Kamuzora, C. L. (1992). Demography of the Hadza, an increasing and high density population of savanna foragers. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 89(2), 159–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., Marlowe, F., Hawkes, K., & O’Connell, J. F. (2000). Paternal investment and hunter-gatherer divorce rates. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons (Eds.), Adaptation and human behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 69–90). New York: Aldine.

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2004). The evolution of strong reciprocity: cooperation in heterogeneous populations. Theoretical Population Biology, 65(1), 17–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1988). The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 132(3), 337–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. (2002). Pre-columbian human impact on California vertebrates: Evidence from old bones and implications for wilderness policy. In C. E. Kay & R. T. Simmons (Eds.), Wilderness and political ecology: Aboriginal influences and the original state of nature (pp. 44–71). Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn, H. (2001). Hunting, power scavenging and butchery by Hadza foragers and by Plio-Pleistocene Homo. In C. B. Stanford & H. T. Bunn (Eds.), Meat-eating and human evolution (pp. 199–217). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn, H., Bartram, L., & Kroll, E. (1988). Variability in bone assemblage formation from Hadza hunting, scavenging, and carcass processing. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 7, 412–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, O., Hamilton, M., & Walker, R. (2005). The prey as patch model: optimal handling of resources with diminishing returns. Journal of Archaeological Science, 32(8), 1147–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cerling, T. E., Wynn, J. G., Andanje, S. A., Bird, M. I., Korir, D. K., Levin, N. E., et al. (2011). Woody cover and hominin environments in the past 6 million years. Nature, 476(7358), 51–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapais, B. (2011). The evolutionary history of pair-bonding and parental collaboration. In C. Salmon & T. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 33–50). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A., & Marlowe, F. (2008). Allomaternal care among the Hadza of Tanzania. Human Nature, 19(3), 249–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damas, D. (1972). Central Eskimo systems of food sharing. Ethnology, 11(3), 220–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flinn, M. V., & Low, B. S. (1986). Resource distribution, social competition, and mating patterns in human societies. In D. I. Rubenstein & R. W. Wrangham (Eds.), Ecological aspects of social evolution: Birds and mammals (pp. 217–243). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flinn, M. V. (2011). Evolutionary anthropology of the human family. In C. Salmon & T. Shackleford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 12–32). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M. (2004). To give or not to give: an evolutionary ecology of human food transfers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 543–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Hill, K. (2009). Why do men hunt? A reevaluation of “man the hunter” and the sexual division of labor. Current Anthropology, 50(1), 51–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Hill, K. (2010). Moving beyond stereotypes of men’s foraging goals. Current Anthropology, 51(2), 265–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. (2001). Reservation food sharing among the Ache of Paraguay. Human Nature, 12(4), 273–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior, I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1990). Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky strategies. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 145–166). Boulder: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: tests of another hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12(1), 29–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (2001). Is meat the hunter’s property? Big game, ownership, and explanations of hunting and sharing. In C. Stanford & H. Bunn (Eds.), Meat-eating and human evolution (pp. 219–236). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1991). Hunting income patterns among the hadza: big game, common goods, foraging goals and evolution of the human diet. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 334(1270), 243–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning, and the evolution of long postmenopausal life spans. Current Anthropology, 38(4), 551–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001a). Hadza meat sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(2), 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001b). Hunting and nuclear families: some lessons from the Hadza about men’s work. Current Anthropology., 42, 681–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J., & Coxworth, J. E. (2010). Family provisioning is not the only reason men hunt. Current Anthropology., 52(2), 259–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helm, J. (1968). The nature of Dogrib socioterritorial groups. In R. Lee & I. Devore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 118–126). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrandt, W., & McGuire, K. (2002). The ascendance of hunting during the California Middle Archaic: an evolutionary perspective. American Antiquity, 67(2), 231–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hawkes, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1987). Foraging decisions among Ache hunter-gatherers: new data and implications for optimal foraging models. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (1988). Macronutrient modifications of optimal foraging theory: an approach using indifference curves applied to some modern foragers. Human Ecology, 16(2), 157–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., Walker, R., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., et al. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331(6022), 1286–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, G. (1978). The food-sharing behavior of protohuman hominids. Scientific American, 238(4), 90–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, A. V., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). The evolution of food sharing in primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(11), 2125–2140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1985). Food sharing among Ache foragers: tests of explanatory hypotheses. Current Anthropology, 26, 223–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(4), 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman, D., & Malcolm, J. (1981). The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. In D. Gubernick & P. Klopfer (Eds.), Parental care in mammals (pp. 347–387). New York: Plenum.

  • Koster, J. M. (2011). Inter-household meat sharing among Mayangna and Miskito horticulturalists in Nicaragua. Human Nature, 22(4), 394–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovejoy, C. O. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 211(4480), 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madrigal, T., & Holt, J. (2002). White-tailed deer meat and marrow return rates and their application to eastern woodlands archaeology. American Antiquity, 67(4), 745–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (1999). Showoffs or providers? The parenting effort of Hadza men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(6), 391–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2003). A critical period for provisioning by Hadza men: implications for pair bonding. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(3), 217–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2004). Mate preferences among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Human Nature, 15(4), 365–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2005). Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 14(2), 54–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2007). Hunting and gathering: the human sexual division of foraging labor. Cross Cultural Research, 41(2), 170–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, D., & Jones, K. (1988). A reconsideration of animal body-part utility indexes. American Antiquity, 53(3), 486–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, S., & DuBois, C. (2002). The anthropology of food and eating. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 99–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Møller, A., & Cuervo, J. (2000). The evolution of paternity and paternal care in birds. Behavioral Ecology, 11(5), 472–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolin, D. (2010). Food-sharing networks in Lamalera, Indonesia: reciprocity, kinship, and distance. Human Nature, 21(3), 243–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1988). Hadza hunting, butchering, and bone transport and their archaeological implications. Journal of Anthropological Research, 44(2), 113–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1990). Reanalysis of large mammal body part transport among the Hadza. Journal of Archaeological Science, 17(3), 301–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 36(5), 461–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., Lupo, K. D., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2002). Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. Journal of Human Evolution, 43(6), 831–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patton, J. (2005). Meat sharing for coalitional support. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 137–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, N. (1993). Demand sharing: reciprocity and the pressure for generosity among foragers. American Anthropologist, 95(4), 860–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, R. J. (2008). Human pair–bonds: evolutionary functions, ecological variation, and adaptive development. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17(5), 227–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins, M. D. (1972). Stone age economics. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J., Newlander, K., White, A., Lemke, A., & Anderson, L. (2010). Early Paleoindian big-game hunting in North America: provisioning or politics? Quaternary International. doi:10.1016/j.quaint.2010.10.027.

  • Tomasello, M., Melis, A., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53, 679–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871-1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungar, P., Grine, F., & Teaford, M. (2006). Diet in early Homo: a review of the evidence and a new model of adaptive versatility. Annual Review of Anthropology, 35, 209–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickery, W., Giraldeau, L., Templeton, J., Kramer, D., & Chapman, C. (1991). Producers, scroungers, and group foraging. American Naturalist, 137(6), 847–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Washburn, S. L., & Lancaster, C. S. (1968). The evolution of hunting. In R. Lee & I. Devore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 293–303). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, G. (1995). Ningiqtuq: resource sharing and generalized reciprocity in Clyde River, Nunavut. Arctic Anthropology, 32(2), 43–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1986). Diet choice, risk, and sharing in a stochastic environment. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5(4), 369–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1996). Marginal model of tolerated theft. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17(1), 37–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. (2006). Prestige or provisioning? A test of foraging goals among the Hadza. Current Anthropology, 47(2), 383–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B., & Marlowe, F. (2010). Hadza social networks. Paper presented at the 109th annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association, November 17-21, New Orleans.

  • Wood, B., & Marlowe, F. (2011). Dynamics of postmarital residence among the Hadza: a kin investment model. Human Nature, 22(1), 128–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1968a). An introduction to Hadza ecology. In R. Lee & I. Devore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 49–55). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1968b). Stability and flexibility in Hadza residential groupings. In R. Lee & I. Devore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 103–117). New York: Aldine.

  • Woodburn, J. (1979). Minimal Politics: The political organization of the Hadza of north Tanzania. In W. Shack & P. Cohen (Eds.), Politics in leadership: A comparative perspective (pp. 244–266). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziker, J., & Schnegg, M. (2005). Food sharing at meals: kinship, reciprocity, and clustering in the Taimyr Autonomous Region, Northern Russia. Human Nature, 16(2), 178–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was made possible by grants from the National Science Foundation, the Leakey Foundation, the Wenner-Gren Foundation, and the Harvard Department of Human Evolutionary Biology. We thank the Tanzanian Commission on Science and Technology for permission to conduct this research. We also thank Dr. Audax Mabulla, Chris Daborn, Claire Porter, Alyssa Crittenden, Johannes and Lena Kleppe, Daudi Peterson, Christian and Nani Schmelling, and Happy Msofe for their help in the field. We appreciate the readers who provided helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript, including Richard Wrangham, Karen Kramer, Douglas Bird, Rebecca Bliege Bird, Kristen Hawkes, James O’Connell, Jeremy Koster, and three anonymous reviewers. Our deepest gratitude is to the Hadza for their cooperation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian M. Wood.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1

(DOCX 50 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wood, B.M., Marlowe, F.W. Household and Kin Provisioning by Hadza Men. Hum Nat 24, 280–317 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9173-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9173-0

Keywords

Navigation