Skip to main content
Log in

Assessing the Role of Sound in the Perception of Food and Drink

  • Published:
Chemosensory Perception

Abstract

The consumption of food and drink are among the most multisensory of our perceptual experiences. In fact, the evaluation of foodstuffs is not only influenced by the unified oral sensation (or Gestalt) of taste and smell in the mouth but also by what the foods look, feel (i.e., oral texture, temperature, viscosity, etc.), and sound like (particularly for noisy foods such as crisps, celery, carrots, etc.) when we eat or drink them. The empirical literature reviewed here highlights the important role that auditory cues have on our perception of a variety of different food and drink items. In particular, changing the sounds that are made when we bite into a foodstuff, or the sounds that are made by carbonated drinks, have both been shown to have a dramatic effect on perception. Auditory stimuli that are not even directly food-related have also been shown to influence people’s behavior. The present article reviews the growing body of research, highlighting the significant effects that music and other auditory stimuli can have on people’s food choices. Taken together, the results of the studies reported here unequivocally show that what people hear (be it food-related or nonrelated sounds) can have a dramatic effect on their perceptions of food and drink. Finally, we report a number of explanations that have been put forward in order to account for these cross-modal effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is worth noting here that auditory information is not the only cue often ignored when people think about their perception of food. The average person would say that the flavour of food derives primarily from its taste in the mouth. They are often surprised to discover that there is a strong ‘nasal’ role in the perception of flavour. In fact, it has been argued that the majority of the flavour of food actually comes from its smell (e.g., Auvray and Spence 2008; Cain 1977; Murphy and Cain 1980; Rozin 1982). The fact that most of our perception of the flavour of food comes from its retronasal smell (or aroma) helps to explain why food appears to lose its taste when your nose is blocked (e.g., by a cold).

  2. One recent influential study of the effect of visual color cues on olfactory perception has shown that colors can have qualitatively different effects on olfactory perception as a function of whether that odor is delivered orthonasally (i.e., from outside the mouth) or retronasally (from inside the mouth; Koza et al. 2005). When colored solutions were presented, participants’ odor intensity ratings were enhanced for odors smelled orthonasally and reduced for odors smelled retronasally (i.e., when the solutions were put in the mouth). The authors suggested that the visual information had less of an influence on the odors presented in the mouth because the retronasal odors are expected to be more salient than orthonasal odors (i.e., odors coming from outside the mouth). The argument is that people pay more attention to food-related stimuli when they are in our mouth than when they are outside the mouth, because one has to decide whether to swallow the food or not when the food is in the mouth. Food-related stimuli outside the mouth are perceived as being less salient; therefore, they are more likely to be influenced by visual cues (i.e., there is likely to be less cost associated with being wrong or rather being “tricked” by the presence of a misleading sensory cue).

References

  • Alais D, Burr D (2004) The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Curr Biol 14:257–262

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Areni CS, Kim D (1993) The influence of background music on shopping behavior: Classical versus top-forty music in a wine store. Adv Consum Res 20:336–340

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashkenazi A, Marks LE (2004) Effect of endogenous attention on detection of weak gustatory and olfactory flavors. Percept Psychophys 66:596–608

    Google Scholar 

  • Auvray M, Spence C (2008) The multisensory perception of flavor. Conscious Cogn 17:1016–1031

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bach PJ, Schaefer JM (1979) The tempo of country music and the rate of drinking in bars. J Stud Alcohol 40:1058–1059

    Google Scholar 

  • Battaglia PW, Jacobs RA, Aslin RN (2003) Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 20:1391–1397

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beeli G, Esslen M, Jäncke L (2005) When coloured sounds taste sweet. Nature 434:38

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beidler LM (1958) Physiological basis of flavor. In: Little AD (ed) Flavor research and food acceptance. Rheinhold, New York, pp 3–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Bult JHF, de Wijk RA, Hummel T (2007) Investigations on multimodal sensory integration: Texture, taste, and ortho- and retronasal olfactory stimuli in concert. Neurosci Lett 411:6–10

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cain WS (1977) History of research on smell. In: Carterette EC, Friedman MP (eds) Handbook of perception: Vol. 6a: tasting and smelling. Academic, New York, pp 197–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) (2004) The handbook of multisensory processes. MIT, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstens E, Cartsens MI, Dessirier J-M, O’Mahony M, Simons CT, Sudo M, Sudo S (2002) It hurts so good: oral irritation by spices and carbonated drinks and the underlying neural mechanisms. Food Qual Prefer 13:431–443

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandrashekar J, Yarmolinsky D, von Buchholtz L, Oka Y, Sly W, Ryba NJ, Zuker CS (2009) The taste of carbonation. Science 326:443–445

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chen J, Karlsson C, Povey M (2005) Acoustic envelope detector for crispness assessment of biscuits. J Texture Stud 36:139–156

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen CM (1980a) Effects of taste quality and intensity on oral perception of viscosity. Percept Psychophys 28:315–320

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen CM (1980b) Effects of solution viscosity on perceived saltiness and sweetness. Percept Psychophys 28:347–353

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen CM, Vickers ZM (1981) Relationships of chewing sounds to judgments of food crispness. J Food Sci 46:574–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clydesdale FM (1993) Color as a factor in food choice. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 33:83–101

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crisinel A-S, Spence C (2009) Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neurosci Lett 464:39–42

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crisinel A-S, Spence C (2010) A sweet sound? Exploring implicit associations between basic tastes and pitch. Perception, in press

  • Dacremont C (1995) Spectral composition of eating sounds generated by crispy, crunchy and crackly foods. J Texture Stud 26:27–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day S (2005) Some demographic and socio-cultural aspects of synesthesia. In: Robertson LC, Sagiv N (eds) Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 11–33

    Google Scholar 

  • de Wijk RA, Engelen L, Prinz JF (2003) The role of intra-oral manipulation in the perception of sensory attributes. Appetite 40:1–7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delwiche J (2004) The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor. Food Qual Prefer 15:137–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake BK (1963) Food crunching sounds. An introductory study. J Food Sci 28:233–241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake B (1965) Food crushing sounds: comparisons of objective and subjective data. J Food Sci 30:556–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake BK (1970) Relationships of sounds and other vibrations to food acceptability. Proceedings of the 3rd International Congress of Food Science and Technology (pp. 437-445). August 9th-14th, Washington D.C.

  • Drews DR, Vaughn DB, Anfiteatro A (1992) Beer consumption as a function of music and the presence of others. J Pa Acad Sci 65:134–136

    Google Scholar 

  • DuBose CN, Cardello AV, Maller O (1980) Effects of colorants and flavorants on identification, perceived flavor intensity, and hedonic quality of fruit-flavored beverages and cake. J Food Sci 45(1393–1399):1415

    Google Scholar 

  • Duizier L (2001) A review of acoustic research for studying the sensory perception of crisp, crunchy and crackly textures. Trends Food Sci Technol 12:17–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncker K (1939) The influence of past experience upon perceptual properties. Am J Psychol 52:255–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429–433

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ferber C, Cabanac M (1987) Influence of noise on gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet or salt. Appetite 8:229–235

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green BG (1992) The effects of temperature and concentration on the perceived intensity and quality of carbonation. Chem Senses 17:435–450

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green BG (1996) Chemesthesis: Pungency as a component of flavor. Trends Food Sci Technol 7:415–424

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK (1998) Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 74:1464–1480

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guéguen N, Le Guellec H, Jacob C (2004) Sound level of background music and alcohol consumption: An empirical evaluation. Percept Mot Skills 99:34–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Guest S, Catmur C, Lloyd D, Spence C (2002) Audiotactile interactions in roughness perception. Exp Brain Res 146:161–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guinard J-X, Mazzucchelli R (1996) The sensory perception of texture and mouthfeel. Trends Food Sci Technol 7:213–219

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hall RL (1958) Flavor study approaches at McCormick and Company, Inc. In: AD Little Inc (ed) Flavor research and food acceptance: a survey of the scope of flavor and associated research, compiled from papers presented in a series of symposia given in 1956–1957. Reinhold, New York, pp 224–240

  • Herrington JD, Capella LM (1996) Effect of music in service environments: A field study. J Serv Mark 10:26–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt-Hansen K (1968) Taste and pitch. Percept Mot Skills 27:59–68

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holt-Hansen K (1976) Extraordinary experiences during cross-modal perception. Percept Mot Skills 43:1023–1027

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jousmäki V, Hari R (1998) Parchment-skin illusion: Sound-biased touch. Curr Biol 8:869–872

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellaris J, Kent R (1992) The influence of music on consumers’ temporal perceptions: Does time fly when you’re having fun. J Consum Psychol 1:365–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellaris J, Mantel S, Altsech MB (1996) Decibels, disposition, and duration: a note on the impact of musical loudness and internal states on time perception. In: Advances in Consumer Research 23:498–503. Association for Consumer Research, Provo

  • Kilcast D (1999) Sensory techniques to study food texture. In: Rosenthal AJ (ed) Food texture: measurement and perception. Aspen, Gaithersburg, pp 30–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Koza BJ, Cilmi A, Dolese M, Zellner DA (2005) Color enhances orthonasal olfactory intensity and reduces retronasal olfactory intensity. Chem Senses 30:643–649

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria AR (1968) The mind of a mnemonist. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks LE, Wheeler ME (1998) Attention and the detectability of weak-taste stimuli. Chem Senses 23:19–29

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Masuda M, Yamaguchi Y, Arai K, Okajima K (2008) Effect of auditory information on food recognition. IEICE Technical Report 108(356):123–126

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarron A, Tierney KJ (1989) The effect of auditory stimulation on the consumption of soft drinks. Appetite 13:155–159

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McElrea H, Standing L (1992) Fast music causes fast drinking. Percept Mot Skills 75:362

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden D, Barr AE, Young RE (1971) Audio analgesia: lack of a cross-masking effect on taste. Percept Psychophys 10:175–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Milliman RE (1986) The influence of background music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. J Consum Res 13:286–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moir HC (1936) Some observations on the appreciation of flavour in foodstuffs. J Soc Chem Ind 14:145–148

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy C, Cain WS (1980) Taste and olfaction: independence vs. interaction. Physiol Behav 24:601–605

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • North AC, Hargreaves DJ (1996) The effects of music on responses to a dining area. J Environ Psychol 16:55–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • North AC, Hargreaves DJ (1998) The effects of music on atmosphere and purchase intentions in a cafeteria. J Appl Soc Psychol 28:2254–2273

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • North A, Hargreaves D (2008) The social and applied psychology of music. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • North AC, Hargreaves DJ, McKendrick J (1997) In-store music affects product choice. Nature 390:132

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • North A, Hargreaves D, McKendrick J (1999) The influence of in-store music on wine selections. J Appl Psychol 84:271–276

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • North AC, Shilcock A, Hargreaves DJ (2003) The effect of musical style on restaurant customers’ spending. Environ Behav 35:712–718

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philipsen DH, Clydesdale FM, Griffin RW, Stern P (1995) Consumer age affects response to sensory characteristics of a cherry flavoured beverage. J Food Sci 60:364–368

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce AH (1907) Gustatory audition; a hitherto undescribed variety of synaesthesia. Am J Psychol 18:341–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Klein RM (1976) Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychol Rev 83:157–171

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott J, Stevenson RJ (1995a) Pungency in food perception and preference. Food Rev Int 11:665–698

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott J, Stevenson RJ (1995b) Effects of oral chemical irritation on tastes and flavors in frequent and infrequent users of chili. Physiol Behav 58:1117–1127

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ragneskog H, Brane G, Karlsson I, Kihlgren M (1996) Influence of dinner music on food intake and symptoms common in dementia. Scand J Caring Sci 10:11–17

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roballey TC, McGreevy C, Rongo RR, Schwantes ML, Steger PJ, Wininger MA, Gardner EB (1985) The effect of music on eating behavior. Bull Psychon Soc 23:221–222

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohde F, Normand MD, Peleg M (1993) Characterization of the power spectrum of force-deformation relationships of crunchy foods. J Texture Stud 24:45–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roudaut G, Dacremont C, Valles Pamies B, Colas B, Le Meste M (2002) Crispness: A critical review on sensory and material science approaches. Trends Food Sci Technol 13:217–227

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin P (1982) “Taste-smell confusions” and the duality of the olfactory sense. Percept Psychophys 31:397–401

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rudmin F, Cappelli M (1983) Tone-taste synesthesia: A replication. Percept Mot Skills 56:118

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour SK, Hamann DD (1988) Crispness and crunchiness of selected low moisture foods. J Texture Stud 19:79–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankar MU, Levitan CA, Prescott J, Spence C (2009a) The influence of color and label information on flavor perception. Chemosens Percept 2:53–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankar MU, Levitan C, Spence C (2009b) Does higher level knowledge mediates the interpretation of multisensory cues? Conscious Cogn. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.008

  • Sherman P, Deghaidy FS (1978) Force-deformation conditions associated with the evaluation of brittleness and crispness in selected foods. J Texture Stud 9:437–459

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith PC, Curnow R (1966) “Arousal hypothesis” and the effects of music on purchasing behavior. J Appl Psychol 50:255–256

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Gallace A (2010) Multisensory design: reaching out to touch the consumer. Psychol Mark, in press

  • Spence C, Shankar MU (2010) The influence of auditory cues on the perception of, and responses to, food and drink. J Sens Stud, in press

  • Spence C, Zampini M (2006) Auditory contributions to multisensory product perception. Acta Acustica 92:1009–1025

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Zampini M (2007) Affective design: modulating the pleasantness and forcefulness of aerosol sprays by manipulating aerosol spraying sounds. CoDesign 3(Supplement 1):109-123

  • Spence C, Shankar MU, Blumenthal H (2010) Sound bites: auditory contributions to the perception and consumption of food and drink. In: Bacci F, Melcher D (eds) Art and the senses. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan M (1955) Has the ear a role in registering flavour? Bulletin of the Central Food Technology Research Institute Mysore (India) 4:136

  • Suzuki Y, Gyoba J, Sakamoto S (2008) Selective effects of auditory stimuli on tactile roughness perception. Brain Res 1242:87–94

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Varela P, Chen J, Karlsson C, Povey M (2006) Crispness assessment of roasted almonds by an integrated approach to texture description: Texture, acoustics, sensory and structure. J Chemometr 20:311–320

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vatakis A, Spence C (2007) Crossmodal binding: Evaluating the “unity assumption” using audiovisual speech stimuli. Percept Psychophys 69:744–756

    Google Scholar 

  • Vatakis A, Spence C (2008) Evaluating the influence of the ‘unity assumption’ on the temporal perception of realistic audiovisual stimuli. Acta Psychol 127:12–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhagen JV, Engelen L (2006) The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: sensory integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:613–650

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1979) Crispness and crunchiness in foods. In: Sherman P (ed) Food texture and rheology. Academic, London, pp 145–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1981) Relationships of chewing sounds to judgments of crispness, crunchiness and hardness. J Food Sci 47:121–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1983) Pleasantness of food sounds. J Food Sci 48:783–786

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1984) Crispness and crunchiness - A difference in pitch? J Texture Stud 15:157–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1985) The relationships of pitch, loudness and eating technique to judgments of the crispness and crunchiness of food sounds. J Texture Stud 16:85–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1987) Crispness and crunchiness - textural attributes with auditory components. In: Moskowitz HR (ed) Food texture: instrumental and sensory measurement. Dekker, New York, pp 145–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM (1988) Evaluation of crispness. In: Blanshard JMV (ed) Food structure: its creation and evaluation. Butterworths, London, pp 433–448

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers Z (1991) Sound perception and food quality. J Food Qual 14:87–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers Z, Bourne MC (1976a) A psychoacoustical theory of crispness. J Food Sci 41:1158–1164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers Z, Bourne MC (1976b) Crispness in foods - A review. J Food Sci 41:1153–1157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers ZM, Wasserman SS (1979) Sensory qualities of food sounds based on individual perceptions. J Texture Stud 10:319–332

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh V, Kulikowski J (1998) Perceptual constancy: why things look as they do. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB, Warren DH (1986) Intersensory interactions. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP (eds) Handbook of perception and performance: Vol 1—Sensory processes and perception. Wiley, New York, pp 25-1–25-36

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley J (1973) Putting colour into marketing. Marketing, October, 24–29, 67

  • Wilson S (2003) The effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Psychol Music 31:93–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yalch R, Spangenberg E (1990) Effects of store music on shopping behavior. J Serv Mark 4:31–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yau NJN, McDaniel MR (1992) Carbonation interactions with sweetness and sourness. J Food Sci 57:1412–1416

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini M, Spence C (2004) The role of auditory cues in modulating the perceived crispness and staleness of potato chips. J Sens Stud 19:347–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini M, Spence C (2005) Modifying the multisensory perception of a carbonated beverage using auditory cues. Food Qual Prefer 16:632–641

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini M, Sanabria D, Phillips N, Spence C (2007) The multisensory perception of flavor: assessing the influence of color cues on flavor discrimination responses. Food Qual Prefer 18:975–984

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini M, Wantling E, Phillips N, Spence C (2008) Multisensory flavor perception: Assessing the influence of fruit acids and color cues on the perception of fruit-flavored beverages. Food Qual Prefer 19:335–343

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Massimiliano Zampini.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zampini, M., Spence, C. Assessing the Role of Sound in the Perception of Food and Drink. Chem. Percept. 3, 57–67 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-010-9064-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-010-9064-2

Keywords

Navigation