Introduction

Dissociation is a complex heterogeneous phenomenon. It has been defined as a “disruption of and/or discontinuity in the normal, subjective integration of one or more aspects of psychological functioning, including – but not limited to – memory, identity, consciousness, perception, and motor control” [1, p. 826]. This definition implicates a wide range of psychological symptoms (e.g., depersonalization, derealization, emotional numbing, and memory fragmentations) and somatoform symptoms (e.g., analgesia) [2,3,4]. Aside from the inability to access normally amenable information and control motor processes (negative symptoms), dissociation includes involuntary intrusions of sensory, affective, and cognitive information into conscious awareness or behavior, e.g., dissociative flashbacks (positive symptoms) [3]. Dissociation can be conceptualized both as a general tendency (trait dissociation) and transient state (state dissociation) and can also be observed in nonclinical populations, albeit at a much lesser extent than in clinical populations [2, 4].

Pathological dissociation is a trans-diagnostic phenomenon, highly prevalent in dissociative disorders and in trauma-related disorders, including depersonalization/derealization disorder (DDD), dissociative identity disorder (DID), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and borderline personality disorder (BPD) [1, 5]. With respect to PTSD, the most recent version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) includes a dissociative subtype (dissociative subtype of posttraumatic stress disorder, D-PTSD), characterized by predominately dissociative responses to traumatic reminders and other stressors in the form of depersonalization and/or derealization [5]. In BPD, dissociation is primarily stress-related and appears to have substantial impact on affective–cognitive functioning [6,7,8].

The precise neural underpinnings of dissociation are still unclear. Yet, neuroimaging research in clinical samples characterized by high dissociation (e.g., DDD, DID, and D-PTSD) have already provided valuable insight into structural and functional networks of brain regions possibly involved in dissociation [9, 10, 11•]. Compared to this relatively large body of literature, neuroimaging studies on dissociation in BPD are still relatively rare.

The present article gives an overview of recent neuroimaging studies in BPD examining associations between state/trait dissociation and altered brain structure and function. Disentangling disorder-specific effects is complicated, as disorders characterized by high dissociation (e.g., BPD, D-PTSD, and dissociative disorders) are highly comorbid and may share etiological factors, such as psychological trauma. Therefore, our present article has two objectives: first, we aim to provide an overview of etiological and neurobiological models of dissociation, primarily based on previous findings in DDD, DID, and D-PTSD. A complete review of this literature is beyond the scope of this article; therefore, for more extensive reviews, see, e.g., [1, 9, 10, 11•]. Our second aim is to discuss recent neuroimaging studies (including measures of state/trait dissociation) in BPD, with respect to key findings related to dissociation, methodological differences and limitations, and possible implications for future research and the clinical setting.

Etiological Models: Trauma and Dissociation

Psychological trauma, stress such as severe and chronic childhood abuse/neglect, has been critically implicated in the development of dissociation [1,13,14,15,16,, 10, 11•, 1217], suggesting a complex interaction of (genetic, neurobiological, and cognitive) predispositions/vulnerabilities and stressful life events [18,19,20].

Dissociation during traumatic events (also referred to as peritraumatic dissociation [18]) can be considered an adaptive defense mechanism to cope with overwhelming threat that cannot be prevented or escaped [3, 11•]. States of subjective detachment (e.g., depersonalization, derealization, and numbing) may help to create an inner distance to the overwhelming experience by dampening unbearable emotions and reducing conscious awareness of the event. The traumatic situation may be perceived as an unreal film-like scene that is not happening to oneself but observed from a wider distance. Somatoform symptoms such as analgesia and out of body experiences (e.g., the sense of floating above one’s body) may reduce awareness of physical injury [16].

While direct translations between animal and human studies are difficult [21], some models have conceptualized peritraumatic dissociation analogous to the freezing response observed in animals (see, e.g., [16]). The proximity of threat may at first elicit an orienting response, preparing the organism for an active defense mechanism (fight or flight reaction [22]), associated with increased sympathetic nervous system activation (e.g., in heart rate, blood pressure, and release of stress hormones). In situations that cannot be controlled or escaped, the threatened organism may more likely engage in a passive defense mode, accompanied by tonic immobility, increased parasympathic activity, and a “shut-down” of the arousal system [14, 16, 21, 23]. Passive reactions (i.e, tonic immobility) in the face of unescapable threat may enhance survival when the chance of escaping or winning a fight is low or impossible, e.g., by reducing the risk of being detected [23, 24]. As pointed out before, however, translations from animal to human research are complicated by conceptual and methodological differences (see, e.g., [21]).

There is evidence that peritraumatic dissociation increases the risk of subsequent PTSD [18,26,27,28,29,, 2530]. Although the precise mechanisms remain elusive [18, 28], disturbed information processing, most prominently memory alterations, may play an important role in this relationship [31,32,33,34]. Dissociation is thought to interfere with a coherent encoding of salient events [35,36,37], leading to a fragmentation (compartmentalization) of memory: sensory, affective, and cognitive aspects of the traumatic event are encoded and stored as separate elements, which may later reoccur as implicit intrusive flashback memories, accompanied by strong sensory impressions as if the traumatic event was happening again in the present [29,39,40,41,, 3842].

Moreover, individuals who are highly vulnerable to experience peritraumatic dissociation are more likely to respond in a similar way to traumatic reminders later on in life [43, 44•, 45]. Dissociation can also develop in the aftermath of trauma and generalize across situations, i.e., individuals who learned to dissociate in response to traumatic/stressful situations may be more likely to do so in the presence of even relatively “minor” stressors [10]. Such trauma-related states of consciousness may comprise distortions in time (e.g., flashback memories), thought (e.g., voice hearing in second-person perspective), body (e.g., depersonalization and out of body experiences), and emotional numbing [43, 44•]. Thereby, dissociation can become maladaptive [10] and possibly interfere with treatment [46,47,48,49,50]. Furthermore, it is assumed that dissociation has an impact on brain function, as discussed in more detail below.

Models on Brain Structure and Function Associated With Dissociation

Up to now, the precise neural/neurobiological underpinnings of dissociation remain elusive. Yet, a growing number of neuroimaging studies in DDD, DID, and D-PTSD have implicated dissociative symptoms in altered brain structure and function.

Over the past decades, neuroimaging has become one of the most important tools in clinical neurobiology. Techniques such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), MR spectroscopy (MRS), positron emission tomography (PET), and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) are used to study abnormalities in the brain. By detecting changes in blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal (hemodynamic response), functional MRI (fMRI) provides a measure of brain activity and coactivity (functional connectivity) during experimental tasks or resting state, i.e., in the absence of experimental stimulation. PET (e.g., in combination with pharmacologic challenge) can be used to detect changes in glucose metabolism and neurotransmitter systems. MRS assesses concentrations of neurochemical metabolites like glutamate, N-acetylaspartate (NAA), lactate, or choline in the brain. Structural MRI and DTI measure anatomical abnormalities, e.g., in gray or white matter volume.

Several neuroimaging studies have related their findings to higher scores on psychometric scales like the dissociative experiences scale (DES), measuring trait dissociation with the subscales depersonalization/derealization, amnesia, and absorption [51], or the dissociation stress scale (DSS), a measure of state dissociation, including items on psychological and somatic dissociation and one item on aversive inner tension [52,53,54]. As an attempt to mimic dissociative experiences in everyday-life situations, some studies used script-driven imagery to experimentally induce dissociation [55,56,57,58,59, 60•]: a narrative of an autobiographical situation involving dissociative experiences (“dissociation script,” as compared to an emotionally neutral script) is created together with each participant and presented in an experimental setting (e.g., during fMRI). Participants are instructed to recall the specific situation as vividly as possible. Other studies used pharmacological challenge (e.g., NMDA antagonists and cannabinoids) to induce dissociative symptoms (see [61]). In the following section, neurobiological models of dissociation, primarily based on research in DDD, DID, and D-PTSD, are discussed.

Cortico-Limbic Disconnection Model and Neuroimaging Research in Depersonalization Disorder

Already in 1998, Sierra and Berrios proposed that symptoms of depersonalization may be associated with a “disconnection” of a cortico-limbic brain system, involving the amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and prefrontal structures. In this model, depersonalization is more broadly conceptualized as a state of subjective detachment, involving emotional numbing, emptiness of thoughts, analgesia, and hypervigilance [62]. It is assumed that these symptoms are associated with increased activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and ACC [63], brain areas implicated in attention, cognitive control, and arousal modulation. Increased recruitment of the PFC may (both directly and indirectly via the ACC) lead to dampened activity in the amygdala and a marked attenuation of automatic responses, comparable to “shutting down the affective system” [67,62,, 6466]. The amygdala is fundamentally involved in salience detection and emotion processing such as the initiation of stress and fear responses [68,63,, 6769]. States of detachment (e.g., numbing) may thus be associated with reduced reactivity in this area [70].

Using fMRI, Phillips and colleagues (2001) investigated brain activity during the presentation of aversive versus neutral images in patients with chronic depersonalization disorder, patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and healthy controls (HC) [69]. In response to aversive images, depersonalization disorder patients reported less arousal and showed diminished activity in the occipito-temporal cortex, ACC, and insula compared to OCD and HCs [69]. The insula plays an important role in attention modulation, encoding of negative emotions, interoceptive awareness, and pain perception [71,72,73,74,75]. Diminished activity in this area may therefore reflect reduced interoceptive/emotional awareness [69, 76•]—an assumption that is supported by a more recent study in chronic depersonalization patients [77•]. In this study by Lemche and colleagues, altered anterior insula and dorsal ACC reactivity to sad emotional expressions was associated with traits of alexithymia, i.e., difficulties in identifying and describing feelings. In another study of this group [78•], a stronger coupling of the dorsomedial PFC (Brodmann area (BA) 9) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; BA31) was found in depersonalization disorder patients [78•]. The PCC is a critical node of the default mode network (DMN), a brain network that has been implicated in “inward-directed” (self-referential) processes, such as episodic memory encoding/retrieval, self-monitoring, daydreaming, planning, rumination, and pain processing [79,80,81]. Further evidence for altered self-referential processing in depersonalization disorder patients stems from an fMRI study in which DDD patients were exposed to either their own photographs or a stranger’s face [82•]. While viewing their own photographs, patients showed stronger activity in areas implicated in self-referential processing, e.g., mPFC, which was positively correlated with depersonalization severity [82•].

Brain function in depersonalization disorder may also be altered in the absence of experimental stimulation: in a PET study by Simeon and colleagues (2000), patients with chronic depersonalization disorder demonstrated significantly lower metabolic activity in the right middle/superior temporal gyrus (BA21/22) and higher metabolism in the parietal and occipital areas (BA7, 39, and 19)—metabolic activity in area 7B was positively correlated with clinical depersonalization scores [83]. Altered glucose metabolism in tempo-parietal regions may play a role in “feeling unreal” [83], e.g., altered consciousness, sensory integration, body schema, and memory, as suggested by observations in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy [84] and research on the role of the temporal lobe in memory processing [32].

In sum, there is evidence for altered activity in brain regions associated with emotional and self-referential processing in patients with chronic depersonalization disorder.

Models on Emotion Modulation and Research in the Dissociative Subtype of PTSD (D-PTSD)

Based on an earlier research in PTSD, Krystal and colleagues (1995) proposed that the thalamus plays an important role in dissociative-like states of altered consciousness. One of the functions of the thalamus is that of a sensory gate or filter, receiving direct and indirect input from sub-cortical areas (e.g., raphe nuclei and locus coeruleus), limbic regions (e.g., amygdala), and frontal areas (e.g., ACC and prefrontal cortices) [85]. Within this network, the thalamus may both directly and indirectly modulate responses to environmental stimuli, facilitating or impeding the flow of information [34, 61, 85].

Furthermore, the hippocampus and parahippocampal regions may be critical to the understanding of altered memory processing during dissociative states [31,32,33, 61].

Based on more recent neuroimaging findings in PTSD [10], Lanius and colleagues (2010) proposed a neurobiological model that distinguishes between two types of responses to traumatic reminders or other stressors: patients with a dissociative response type (D-PTSD) are thought to “over-modulate” their emotions, as opposed to patients who primarily suffer from re-experiencing symptoms, including (affective) hyperarousal, intense feelings of shame and guilt, and difficulties in emotion downregulation (re-experiencing response type). Emotion over-modulation (dissociative response type) is thought to primarily activate frontal regions implicated in cognitive control and emotion downregulation (e.g., dorsal/rostral ACC and mPFC), associated with dampened activity in amygdala and insula. The reversed pattern—diminished frontal recruitment (ACC and mPFC) and hyperactivity in amygdala and insula—is assumed to underlie emotion under-modulation (re-experience response type) [10].

Central to the development of this model [10] was a script-driven imagery fMRI study [57], in which PTSD patients were exposed to autobiographical narratives of traumatic events. The majority of patients (∼70%) reported marked re-experiencing symptoms and showed a substantial increase in heart rate during the script. In a smaller patient group (∼30%), however, this heart rate increase was not observed—instead, these patients showed stronger activity in the medial frontal gyrus, anterior and medial cingulate, middle temporal gyri (BA38), precuneus, occipital areas, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), compared to a control group of traumatized persons who had not developed PTSD [57].

In another fMRI study [86], patients with D-PTSD showed increased activity in the amygdala, insula, and thalamus while fearful versus neutral facial expressions were presented nonconsciously. Interestingly, these limbic(-related) areas were not significantly activated when images were presented consciously. In the latter case, dissociative patients showed increased activity in ventral PFC and diminished activity in the dorsomedial PFC, suggesting a conscious over-modulation of emotions and suppression of self-referential processing [86].

For PTSD patients who showed dissociative responses to autobiographical trauma scripts (compared to patients with a flashback response and healthy controls), there is also evidence for altered functional connectivity (FC), i.e., coactivation in areas implicated in sensory processing, consciousness, memory, and emotion regulation [56]: compared to controls, dissociative patients showed stronger FC of the left ventrolateral thalamus (VLT) with right insula, middle frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, cuneus, and with left parietal lobe, but reduced VLT-FC with the left superior frontal gyrus, right parahippocampal gyrus, and right superior occipital gyrus. Compared to patients with a flashback response, dissociative patients showed increased FC between right cingulate gyrus and left IFG [56].

In the absence of experimental tasks, altered resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) in the DMN was found in patients with D-PTSD [87•], including altered synchrony between the DMN (which is usually activated during rest) and the “central executive network” (commonly activated during cognitive tasks, as reflected in strong anticorrelations [75, 88]). Findings of altered intra-network resting-state connectivity (in DMN) and altered inter-network connectivity were significantly associated with depersonalization and derealization severity [87•].

In another RS-fMRI study [89•], patients with D-PTSD (compared to PTSD patients without the dissociative subtype and HC) demonstrated increased FC of the amygdala with prefrontal and parietal regions, including dorsal PCC and precuneus, which may further support the assumption of a distinct “neurobiological profile” of D-PTSD [89•].

Research in Dissociative Identity Disorder (DID)

There is some evidence that the aforementioned neurobiological alterations may not be specific to a specific disorder but rather represent a trans-diagnostic phenomenon related to dissociation. Recent findings in DID [90•, 91•] resemble findings for D-PTSD, albeit intra-individual differences (instead of inter-individual differences) were observed: neurobiological responses significantly differed depending on whether DID patients were in a “hyper-aroused traumatic identity state” (with voluntary access to traumatic memories) or in their “normal dissociative identity state” (characterized by dissociative amnesia) [90•, 91•]. In the two studies by Reinders and colleagues (2006, 2014), DID patients showed elevated cardiovascular responses (heart rate and blood pressure) and stronger amygdala and insula activity, along with lower activity in cingulate gyrus, parietal cortex, and parahippocampus when exposed to a trauma script (versus neutral script) while being in their “hyper-aroused traumatic identity state” than in their neutral “hypo-aroused identity state” [90•, 91•]. In another study, DID patients exhibited increased perfusion in bilateral thalamus while being in their (apparently) “normal” state of identity compared to an (apparently) “emotional” identity state [92•]. More research is needed to clarify whether brain activity patterns may differ dependent on states (in the same individuals) or represent stable inter-individual differences, which may allow for a discrimination between diagnostic subgroups/categories [10].

Research on Structural Alterations

Aside from functional alterations, several studies reported structural abnormalities in clinical samples with high trait dissociation, although these structural findings are still quite heterogeneous.

In depersonalization disorder, reduced gray matter volumes (GMV) in right thalamus, caudate, and cuneus, and increased GMV in the left dorsomedial PFC and the right somato-sensoric regions were observed [93•]. As abovementioned, these areas have been implicated in dissociation [10, 61, 62, 85].

In DID, reduced volumes in the amygdala and hippocampus [94, 95] and parahippocampus [95] were found, although discrepant findings of normal amygdala and hippocampal volumes compared to healthy controls were also reported [96]. Smaller hippocampal volumes may be related to early life trauma: the hippocampus has a high density of glucocorticoid receptors and is highly sensitive to a heightened release of the stress hormone cortisol—therefore, chronic traumatic stress may lead to cell damage in this area [31,32,33,34]. Smaller hippocampal volumes were also found in healthy individuals with childhood trauma, who did not develop a disorder [97]. Reduced hippocampal volumes in PTSD [98•, 99] may therefore stem from a history of trauma rather than specific to the diagnosis [100]. In a recent study [98•], comparing PTSD patients with versus without dissociative subtype, no significant group differences in amygdala, hippocampus, and parahippocampus volumes were observed. Yet, patients with D-PTSD showed increased GMV in right precentral and fusiform gyri and reduced GMV in right inferior temporal gyrus. Severity of depersonalization and derealization was positively correlated with GMV in the right middle frontal gyrus [98•]. Another study in PTSD [101•] found positive associations between trait dissociation and GMVs in medial/lateral PFC, orbitofrontal, temporal polar, parahippocampal, and inferior parietal cortices—brain regions associated with emotion regulation.

Extending findings on GMV alterations, a recent study in dissociative disorders [102•] found significantly lower fractional anisotropy in white matter of the right anterior corona radiate (which receives projections from the basal ganglia) compared to healthy controls. More research is needed to understand how these alterations may be related to specific clinical symptoms of dissociation.

As already pointed out in the context of functional neuroimaging studies, interpretation of structural studies may be complicated by the presence of comorbidities. Patients with comorbid PTSD+DID showed significantly larger volumes of the putamen and pallidum than PTSD patients without DID [103]. Volumes of these regions (implicated in motor control [103,104,105]) were negatively correlated with severity of derealization/depersonalization [103]. Patients with PTSD+DID (but not PTSD patients without DID) further showed smaller hippocampal volumes than healthy controls [103].

Further studies with clinical control groups are needed to gain more insight into this relationship. Of note, structural alterations do not necessarily reflect functional alterations, i.e., more frequent engagement of specific brain areas does not have to be reflected in larger volume of this region and vice versa. Studies including both functional and structural measures may give additional insight into this relationship [106].

Interim Summary

In sum, theoretical assumptions and research in depersonalization/DDD, DID, and D-PTSD suggest a link between dissociative symptoms and alterations in brain regions associated with emotion processing and memory (amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, and middle/superior temporal gyrus), attention and interoceptive awareness (insula), filtering of sensory input (thalamus), self-referential processes (PCC, precuneus, and mPFC), cognitive control, and arousal modulation (IFG, ACC, and lateral prefrontal cortices). As many studies did not include clinical control groups or groups of traumatized individuals who did not develop a disorder, it remains unclear whether the aforementioned results are related to a specific disorder or a trans-diagnostic feature, possibly associated with high dissociation. Findings that are based on correlations (e.g., between brain structure/function and psychometric scores) do not allow causal conclusions, i.e., whether they represent a predisposition for or a result of frequent dissociative experiences (see below).

The second aim of our article is to review neuroimaging studies in BPD that investigated links to dissociative symptoms. We searched databases (PubMed, PsychInfo, Web of Science, and Science Direct) for different combinations of “Borderline Personality Disorder,” “Dissociation,” and the following keywords: brain, brain alterations, functional magnetic resonance imaging, magnetic resonance imaging, neurobiological, neuroimaging, neurophysiological, positron emission tomography, and structural magnetic resonance imaging.

In the next section, we first describe clinical expressions of dissociation in BPD, providing a background for the subsequent discussion of neuroimaging research.

Dissociation in Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD)

Transient stress-related dissociation is a hallmark of BPD [6, 11•, 107]. It has been defined as one of the nine diagnostic criteria for the disorder in DSM-IV [108]. In DSM-V [5], “dissociative states under stress” are still listed among other BPD key features such as emotion dysregulation, instable cognition, impulsivity, and interpersonal disturbances.

Emotion dysregulation in BPD (i.e., heightened sensitivity to emotional stimuli, intense emotions, rapid mood swings, and lack of functional emotion regulation strategies) can have detrimental effects on goal-directed behaviors in every-day life [6, 11•, 13, 109•]. Numerous studies suggest that a dysfunctional network of fronto-limbic brain regions, including a hyperreactivity of the amygdala and insula, and diminished recruitment of frontal regions (e.g., orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), mPFC, and dlPFC) during emotional challenge may underlie emotion dysregulation in BPD (e.g., see [110•, 111•, 112•]).

Stress-related dissociation occurs in about 75–80% of BPD patients [6,114,115,116,117,, 113118], typically lasting between minutes and hours, or days [119, 120]. The strength, frequency, and intensity of dissociative experiences are positively correlated to self-reported arousal/stress levels [6].

Research in BPD has found reasonably strong relationships between dissociation and childhood trauma, especially sexual abuse [118,122,123,, 121124], physical abuse, attachment difficulties, and parental neglect [118, 125, 126] (for an overview, see [11•]).

It has been proposed that stress-related dissociation in BPD may be a form of emotion modulation (e.g., increased attempts to inhibit emotions), comparable to observations in D-PTSD, especially in patients with severe childhood trauma [11•, 127]. By interfering with mental resources that are crucial to cognitive functioning [55, 60•], stress-related dissociation may hinder recovery [128]. BPD patients with high trait dissociation showed significant impairments across multiple neuropsychological domains (including memory, attention, and interference inhibition) [8] [129•]. Recent neuroimaging studies further suggest a substantial impact of experimentally induced dissociation on affective–cognitive functioning in BPD [55, 60•], as discussed in more detail below.

Neuroimaging Research on Dissociation in BPD

To our knowledge, so far, only relatively few neuroimaging studies in BPD examined links between trait/state dissociation and brain function during resting state (RS) [130,132,133,•, 131134] or experimental tasks [135,136,137,138,139,140, 141•], and even fewer studies have directly investigated the impact of experimentally induced dissociation on neural processing [55, 59, 60•]. In the following section, we provide an overview of neuroimaging studies, revealed by our literature search, and recent unpublished research from our group. Table 1 gives an overview of these studies (n = 20), summarizing key results related to dissociation and methodological characteristics (sample characteristics, medication status, trauma history, comorbidity, and measures). In all studies, BPD was assessed according to DSM-IV [108].

Table 1 Overview of the studies on possible links between brain function, brain structure, and dissociation in borderline personality disorder (BPD)

Brain Function During Rest: PET, SPECT, and RS-fMRI Studies

Lange and colleagues used 18fluoro-2-deoxyglucose (FDG-)PET to investigate glucose metabolism in 17 BPD patients (with a history of childhood sexual/physical abuse, mixed gender, partly medicated; see Table 1) and 9 healthy controls (HCs) [131]. BPD patients displayed reduced FDG uptake in the right temporal pole, anterior fusiform gyrus, and left precuneus and PCC. Impaired memory performance among patients was correlated with metabolic activity in ventromedial and lateral temporal cortices (implicated in episodic memory consolidation and retrieval), while no correlations with trait dissociation (DES) were reported. The finding of decreased temporo-parietal metabolism was discussed as possible neural underpinning of altered memory processes that may also play a role in the context of dissociation [131]. However, sample sizes were relatively small, and findings may not be specific to BPD due to comorbidities (depersonalization disorder, DID, and PTSD).

Sar et al. [132] used single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) with Tc99m-hexamethylpropylenamine (HMPAO) as a tracer to investigate regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in an unmedicated sample of DID patients (n = 21), 15 of whom met criteria for comorbid BPD, and 9 HCs. Compared to HCs, patients showed decreased rCBF ratio in bilateral medial OFC and increased rCBF in medial/superior frontal regions and occipital regions bilaterally [63]. No significant correlation with dissociation was reported.

Wolf and colleagues (2012) used continuous arterial spin labeling MRI to measure alterations in blood flow in 16 female BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (partly medicated but on a stable medication) and 16 HCs [133]. Compared to HCs, patients exhibited decreased blow flow in the medial OFC, while increased blood flow was found in the lateral OFC bilaterally. Like in the study by Sar and colleagues [132], no significant correlation with self-reported dissociation (DSS) was observed. Instead, medial and lateral OFC blood flow was positively correlated with impulsivity, as measured with the Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS; [142])—suggesting an association with impulsivity rather than with dissociation.

In 17 female BPD patients without PTSD and 17 HCs (same sample as in [133]), Wolf and colleagues (2011) investigated changes in RSFC using RS-fMRI [134]. Within the DMN, patients showed increased RSFC in the left frontopolar cortex and insula, while showing decreased RSFC in the left cuneus. Within a fronto-parietal network, patients exhibited decreased RSFC in the left inferior parietal lobule and the right middle temporal cortex compared to HCs. No significant group differences were observed in two other networks comprising lateral prefrontal and cingulate regions. In the BPD group, state dissociation (DSS) positively predicted RSFC in the insula and precuneus [134]—as previously mentioned, these regions play a role in interoceptive awareness and self- referential processes [74].

In another RS-fMRI study, Krause-Utz et al. (2014) investigated the changes in RSFC of the three fronto-limbic core regions that are of specific relevance to BPD: bilateral amygdala (medial temporal lobe network), bilateral dorsal ACC (salience network), and bilateral ventral ACC (default mode network) [130•]. The DES was used to predict RSFC with these seed regions. The sample comprised 20 unmedicated female BPD patients (all with a history of interpersonal trauma) and 17 HCs. There was a trend for increased amygdala RSFC with right dorsal insula, lateral OFC, and putamen in patients compared to HCs. BPD patients further exhibited diminished negative RSFC between the dorsal ACC and the left PCC and increased negative RSFC of the left ventral ACC with occipital cortex, lingual gyrus, and cuneus. In the patient group, DES scores positively predicted amygdala RSFC with the right dlPFC. Furthermore, patients, who reported higher trait dissociation showed a reduced coupling of the amygdala with right fusiform gyrus. A subgroup comparison of BPD patients with (n = 9) versus without comorbid PTSD (n = 11) yielded similar findings.

The two RS-fMRI studies described above [130•, 134] provide primary evidence for a possible role of dissociation in altered RSFC in BPD. As pointed out before, no causal conclusions can be drawn from correlational findings. In order to gain more insight into the impact of dissociation on RSFC of large-scale brain networks in BPD, future studies may acquire resting-state scans before and after experimentally inducing dissociation (e.g., via script-driven imagery) and before and after therapeutic interventions aimed at a reduction of dissociative symptoms.

Again, interpretation of findings, summarized so far, is complicated by methodical differences (e.g., comorbidities with depersonalization disorder [131], DID [131, 132], and PTSD [130•]), trauma history [130•, 131], psychotropic medication [131, 133, 134], and relatively small sample sizes.

Neurochemical Alterations: MRS Studies

To our knowledge, one MRS study in BPD investigated links between trait dissociation (DES) and glutamate levels in the ACC within a sample of unmedicated female BPD patients (n = 30) and HCs (n = 30) [143]. Significantly higher levels of glutamate in the ACC were found in BPD as compared with HCs. In BPD, glutamate concentrations in the ACC were positively correlated not only to DES scores, but also to BIS scores. Associations between ACC glutamate levels and impulsivity (BIS scores) could be replicated in a more recent study [144], suggesting a link with impulsivity rather than with dissociation.

Task-Related fMRI Studies

A couple of fMRI studies in BPD examined links between changes in BOLD response during experimental tasks (e.g., viewing aversive images, pain stimulation, and cognitive tasks) and self-reported dissociation (e.g., DSS and DES).

Kraus and colleagues (2009) investigated amygdala activity in relation to pain processing (heat stimulation) and state dissociation (DSS) in an unmedicated sample of female BPD patients with (n = 12) and without comorbid PTSD (n = 17) [137]. No significant group differences in pain sensitivity were observed. A deactivation of the amygdala during pain processing was found to be more pronounced in BPD patients with comorbid PTSD than in those without PTSD, while this was not significantly correlated with DSS scores.

In a more recent study, pain processing and dissociation (DES and DSS before/after scanning) were investigated in relation to FC changes in an unmedicated female sample of BPD patients (n = 25, all with a history of self-harm) and 23 HCs [136]. Kluetsch and colleagues [136] used psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis [136] and independent component analysis to analyze changes in FC. Compared to controls, patients showed a reduced integration of the left retrosplenial cortex and left superior frontal gyrus into the DMN. During pain versus neutral temperature stimulation, patients further exhibited less FC between the PCC (seed region) and left dlPFC. Higher trait dissociation (DES) was associated with an attenuated signal decrease of the DMN in response to painful stimulation. Since only patients with a history of self-harm and no clinical control group were included, future studies should clarify whether these findings can be replicated in BPD patients without self-injurious behavior and whether they may underlie altered pain processing (e.g., analgesia) during dissociation.

Wingenfeld and colleagues (2009) applied an individualized emotional stroop task (EST, with neutral, general negative, and individual negative words) to investigate changes in BOLD signal during emotional interference in 20 BPD patients (partly medicated, mixed gender; see Table 1) and 20 HCs [140]. State dissociation (DSS) was assessed before and after scanning. Patients displayed overall slower reaction times than HCs, while no increase of reaction times after emotional interference was observed. Controls, but not BPD patients, showed a significant recruitment of the ACC and frontal areas for generally negative versus neutral and for individual negative versus neutral words, respectively. No significant correlations between DSS and behavioral measures or neural activity were reported.

Hazlett and colleagues (2012) investigated potentiated amygdala responses to repeatedly presented emotional pictures in an unmedicated sample of BPD patients (n = 33), schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) patients (n = 28), and 32 HCs (mixed gender; see Table 1) [135]. Participants underwent event-related fMRI scanning while viewing repeated (versus novel) neutral, pleasant, and unpleasant pictures. BPD patients demonstrated increased amygdala reactivity to repeated emotional, but not neutral pictures, and a prolonged return to baseline of amygdala activity across all conditions. Despite amygdala overactivation, BPD patients showed blunted arousal ratings of emotional, but not neutral pictures. A significant negative correlation between self-reported dissociation and amygdala activity was found in BPD and also in the SPD group: higher self-reported dissociation (DES) was associated with lower emotion-challenged amygdala reactivity. This latter finding is in line with the assumption that dissociation may serve as a defensive mechanism for unpleasant stimuli (see above).

The influence of dissociation on brain activity and FC during emotional distraction in the context of a working memory task was investigated in two fMRI studies by Krause-Utz and colleagues [139, 145•]. Twenty-two unmedicated female BPD patients (all with a history of interpersonal trauma) and 22 HCs performed a modified Sternberg item-recognition task (emotional working memory task, EWMT, with neutral versus negative interpersonal pictures versus no distractors presented during the delay interval between encoding and retrieval). Immediately before and after the EWMT, state dissociation (DSS) was measured. Patients showed significantly increased amygdala activation and impaired task performance during distraction by negative and neutral interpersonal pictures but not in the control condition without distractors, suggesting increased susceptibility to social cues in BPD [139]. Patients who reported a stronger increase of state dissociation (DSS) showed significantly lower amygdala reactivity to negative distractors [139], in line with aforementioned findings by Hazlett and colleagues [135] and theoretical models [10]. No significant differences were observed between BPD patients with (n = 9) versus without comorbid PTSD.

In a reanalysis of this data set, Krause-Utz, Elzinga, and colleagues (2014) further examined how activity in the bilateral amygdala (seed region) was correlated to activity in other regions across the brain [145•] using PPI [146]. In the BPD group, a stronger coupling of the amygdala with hippocampus and dorsomedial PFC was observed for negative versus no distractors, suggesting an increased coactivation (information exchange) of regions involved in emotional and self-referential processing, e.g., retrieval of autobiographical memories [145•]. During negative distractors, patients with higher state dissociation (DSS4) showed stronger bilateral amygdala FC with right thalamus, right ACC, left insula, and left precentral gyrus—regions implicated in arousal modulation, body movements, attention, and filtering of sensory information [145•].

In another fMRI study, Krause-Utz, Mauchnik, and colleagues [138] investigated the neural correlates of classical fear conditioning (using a differential delay aversive conditioning paradigm with an electric shock as unconditioned stimulus and two neutral pictures as CS− and CS+) in 27 unmedicated female BPD patients and 26 HCs. Controls but not BPD patients showed amygdala habituation during acquisition of CS+paired (CS+ in temporal contingency with the aversive event). No significant effect of trait or state dissociation (DSS) was observed, neither on skin conductance response (SCR) nor on brain activity. In contrast to this, an earlier study by Ebner-Priemer and colleagues (2009) found diminished fear conditioning in terms of SCR and subjective ratings in BPD patients with high (compared to those with low) pre-experimental dissociation [7]. These discrepancies may be related to methodological differences (e.g., assessment of skin conductance during fMRI versus in the laboratory and different sample characteristics: patients in the study by Krause-Utz et al. [106] reported relatively low naturally occurring state dissociation).

Script-Driven Imagery Studies

To our knowledge, three fMRI studies in BPD so far have used script-driven imagery to experimentally induce dissociative states [55, 59, 60•].

In a pilot fMRI study, Ludascher and colleagues [120] investigated dissociation and pain sensitivity in unmedicated female BPD patients with (n = 10) and without comorbid PTSD (n = 5) [59]. During fMRI, patients were exposed to an autographical dissociation script versus a neutral script. After the dissociation script, DSS scores were significantly increased, indicating a successful experimental manipulation, and pain sensitivity was lower than after the neutral script. During the dissociation script, patients further showed increased activation in the left IFG (BA9). Scores on the DSS positively predicted activation in the left superior frontal gyrus (BA6) and negatively predicted activation in the right middle temporal gyrus (BA21) and inferior temporal gyrus (BA20). In the subgroup of patients with BPD+PTSD (n = 10), increased activity in the left cingulate gyrus (BA32) was observed during the dissociation script. Further, DSS scores were positively correlated to the bilateral insula activity (BA13) and negatively correlated with the right parahippocampal gyrus (BA35) activity. However, the sample size was relatively small and no control group was included.

In a subsequent study, Winter and colleagues (2015) investigated the impact of dissociation on cognitive control of emotional material, by combining script-driven imagery with an EST (with negative, neutral, and positive words), a free word recall task, and a recognition task for EST words [60•]. Dissociation was induced in 19 unmediated female BPD patients (BPDd), while 18 BPD patients (BPDn) and 19 HCs were exposed to a neutral script. Script-driven imagery again successfully increased dissociation (DSS) in the BPDd group. During the EST, BPDd patients showed overall cognitive impairments (more errors and slower reaction times) and prolonged reaction times in response to negative versus neutral words compared with the other groups. In the subsequent memory task, the BPDd group recalled fewer EST words and needed more time to recognize words than BPDn. Further, BPDd demonstrated increased activity in left IFG and dlPFC during negative versus neutral words, possibly reflecting increased attempts to inhibit (emotional) material during dissociation.

Recently, Krause-Utz and colleagues (submitted) combined script-driven imagery with the EWMT to study the effect of dissociation on amygdala activity and FC during emotional distraction [55]. Within an unmedicated female sample of 29 BPD patients, dissociation was induced in 17 patients (BPDd), while 12 patients (BPDn) and 18 HC were exposed to a neutral script. Script-driven imagery again successfully increased dissociation (DSS) and aversive pictures within the EWMT evoked significant changes in amygdala activity. Overall, the BPDd patients showed significantly more working memory impairments (incorrect responses and misses, suggesting disturbed memory encoding/retrieval), and reduced activity in bilateral amygdala. Amygdala hyperactivity to emotional pictures (relative to HCs) could only be observed in BPDn. During negative distractors (versus no distractors), BPDd patients further showed lower activity in left cuneus, lingual gyrus, and PCC than BPDn and increased left IFG activity than HC. Increased left IFG activity in BPDd resembles findings of previous studies [59, 60•], albeit it was also found in the BPDn group [55]. The PPI revealed a significantly stronger coupling of amygdala with the left inferior parietal lobule, right superior/middle temporal gyrus, right middle occipital gyrus, and left claustrum during negative distractors in the BPDd group compared to the other groups [55]. More studies, including clinical control groups and larger samples, are needed to gain a deeper insight into the impact of dissociation on neural processes during affective–cognitive processing.

Possible Effects of fMRI Neurofeedback

Paret and colleagues [141•] used real-time fMRI to investigate the effects of a neurofeedback training task on amygdala activity and amygdala-PFC FC. In four training sessions, female BPD patients (n = 8) were instructed to downregulate emotional responses to aversive images based on feedback from a thermometer display, showing amygdala BOLD signals [141•]. The DSS was applied at the end of each training run. Amygdala-PFC FC was altered across the four sessions, with increased amygdala-vlPFC FC for regulating versus passively viewing aversive pictures. Interestingly, self-reported “lack of emotional awareness” (difficulties in emotion regulation scale [147]) and also state dissociation (DSS) decreased with training [148].

Structural Neuroimaging Studies in BPD

A large number of neuroimaging studies in BPD used techniques like structural MRI or DTI to investigate volumetric alterations in comparison to HCs (e.g., see [110•, 112•]). Some of these studies included psychometric scales like the DES or DSS to link their findings to dissociation [149, 150•, 151].

Irle et al. [149] used structural MRI to investigate volumes of the superior (precuneus and postcentral gyrus) and inferior parietal cortices in 30 female BPD patients (all with a history of severe sexual and physical abuse, partly medicated; see Table 1), and 25 HCs. Comorbid dissociative disorders were determined using the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV dissociative disorders (SCID-D) [152]. BPD patients with comorbid dissociative amnesia (DA) or DID had significantly increased left postcentral gyrus volumes compared to HCs (+13%) and BPD patients without these disorders (+11%). In the entire BPD sample, smaller right-sided precuneus volumes (−9%) were observed (compared to HCs) and stronger depersonalization was related to larger right precuneus size, suggesting a possible relationship between dissociative symptoms and volumetric alterations in this region.

Niedtfeld and colleagues (2013) used voxel-based morphometry to investigate structural alterations across the entire brain in a female sample of 60 unmedicated BPD patients with (n = 21) and without comorbid PTSD (n = 31; mainly following severe childhood trauma such as physical/sexual abuse), and HCs (n = 60) [150•]. In BPD, smaller GMV of right amygdala, hippocampus, ACC, fusiform, and inferior temporal gyrus were found. In patients with comorbid BPD+PTSD, increased GMV in the dlPFC and superior temporal gyrus were observed. For a smaller subsample of 42 BPD patients, scores on the DES could be included, which predicted larger GMV in the middle temporal gyrus [150•]—a region previously implicated in dissociation [83, 84].

Extending the abovementioned findings on GMV alterations, Rusch and colleagues (2007) used DTI to study white matter alterations in 20 female unmedicated BPD patients and 20 HCs [151]. Mean diffusivity in inferior frontal white matter was associated with higher trait dissociation (DES), but also with measures of general psychopathology. As all BPD patients had comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), it also remains unclear whether these alterations are specific for BPD.

Interim Summary

To sum up at this point, structural studies on dissociation in BPD are still relatively rare and heterogeneous. Task-related fMRI studies suggest a role of dissociation in brain regions that play an important role in emotion processing/regulation, pain processing, and impulse control, including the amygdala, medial temporal lobe, insula, fusiform gyrus, precuneus, IFG, ACC, and cortical structures (e,g., dlPFC) [110•, 111•, 112•]. These brain regions and (part of) their functions are summarized in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A schematic overview of brain regions and functions (among others) associated with dissociation in borderline personality disorder. The precise neurobiological underpinnings of dissociation remain elusive, but there is evidence for a link between dissociative states/traits and altered (co)activity in brain networks involved in emotion processing and memory (e.g., amygdala and hippocampus/medial temporal lobe memory system), interoception and attention regulation (insula), self-referential processes (e.g., posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus), cognitive control, and arousal modulation (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), inferior frontal gyrus, and anterior cingulate cortex)—functions which may be altered during dissociation

As previously mentioned, the amygdala is fundamentally involved in emotion processing and the initiation of stress and fear responses [68,63,, 6769] and activity in this area might be dampened during dissociation [10, 62]. Three studies in BPD [55, 135, 139] found lower activity in this region in patients reporting higher dissociation, while other studies did not [60•, 137, 138]. These discrepancies may partly be related to differences in study design: studies observing negative correlations studied amygdala reactivity to highly aversive (trauma-related) pictures [55, 135, 139], while Kraus and colleagues [137] studied amygdala activation during pain stimulation and Winter et al. [60•] used emotional words, which might have been less distressing than aversive pictures (not evoking significant changes in amygdala activity). Naturally occurring dissociative states (DSS4 ratings: mean ± standard deviation (M ± SD)) were relatively low ([137]: BPD 1.16 ± 0.28; [138] 2.14 ± 1.67) as compared to script-driven imagery studies inducing dissociation (e.g., [55]: BPDd 6.85 ± 2.03; 9-point Likert scale, 9 indicating extreme symptoms). More research is needed to examine whether effects of dissociation on “affective brain regions” (e.g., amygdala) may become particularly evident in highly stressful situations and during high state dissociation in BPD. This may have implications for neurobiological conceptualizations of the disorder, as amygdala hyperreactivity to emotional stimuli is assumed to be a key feature of BPD [110•, 111•]. Assuming that dissociation dampens amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli, the presence of dissociative symptoms may partly (aside from other factors such as medication status [111•]) explain why some previous studies did not replicate amygdala hyperreactivity in BPD [110•].

Apart from the amygdala, the left IFG, which has been implicated in interference inhibition and suppression of impulses [104], may be implicated in dissociative states in BPD. Three script-driven imagery studies consistently found increased activity in this area in patients listening to a dissociation script [59] and performing affective–cognitive tasks after dissociation induction [55, 60•]. As no clinical control groups were included in these studies and a high percentage of patients [59, 60•] or all patients [55] respectively reported a history of trauma, it remains unclear whether these findings are specific to BPD. Increased activity in the IFG [10] and stronger IFG FC with right cingulate gyrus [131] was also observed in D-PTSD exposed to autobiographical trauma scripts [57].

In addition, there is primary evidence for an association between trait dissociation and alterations in regions of the DMN (subsystems) such as the PCC, precuneus, hippocampus, and dorsal PFC in BPD. As previously mentioned, the DMN has been associated with “inward-directed” (self-referential) processes [79,80,81]. Again, it remains unclear whether these alterations in FC are specific to BPD or rather a trans-diagnostic phenomenon, as alterations in DMN (regions) were also found in depersonalization disorder [78•] and D-PTSD [87•].

Overall Discussion and Conclusion

In this article, we aimed to (1) provide an overview of neurobiological models of dissociation and neuroimaging research in depersonalization disorder, DID, and D-PTSD and (2) to give an overview of recent neuroimaging studies in BPD that examined links between dissociation and altered brain function/structure.

Pathological dissociation is a complex and heterogeneous phenomenon [2,3,4, 153], which has been closely linked to traumatic stress [1, 10]. Dissociation may be a protective strategy to cope with overwhelming emotions in traumatic/stressful situations [1, 3, 10]. The cost of this subjective detachment appears to be a disruption of mental functions that are crucial to the development of identity, self-control, and emotion regulation [1, 16, 36, 37, 116].

The precise neurobiological underpinnings of dissociation remain elusive, but there is evidence for a link between dissociative states/traits and altered (co)activity in brain regions involved in emotion processing and memory (e.g., amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, and middle/superior temporal gyrus), interoception and attention regulation (insula), self-referential processes (e.g., PCC and precuneus), cognitive control, and arousal modulation (e.g., mPFC, IFC, and ACC)—functions which may be altered during dissociation [1, 9, 10, 11•].

Future studies may address how changes in brain activity during dissociation are related to altered neuropsychological/cognitive functioning, e.g., encoding and subsequent recall of emotional information [110•, 111•]. It has been proposed that dissociation is associated with diminished recollection of trauma-related emotional information, although heterogeneous findings were reported [154,155,156]. The combination of dissociation induction and affective–cognitive neuropsychological tasks in neuroimaging research may contribute to a better understanding of this relationship.

As pointed out before, interpretations of the abovementioned research are complicated by methodological aspects (e.g., comorbidities, shared etiological factors, psychotropic medication, and differences in gender distribution) but also by conceptual differences: Dissociation involves a broad range of psychological and somatoform symptoms [2,3,4, 153]. A more precise differentiation between dissociative symptoms in future neuroimaging research may help to identify more specific associations [44•].

Most studies, summarized in this article, used well-established psychometric scales of dissociation (e.g., DES or DSS), showing excellent internal consistency, reliability, and high specificity and sensitivity to change in symptomatology [53]. As mentioned above, correlations (e.g., between neuroimaging findings and scores on these scales) give an estimate of the strength of a relationship (e.g., between brain structure/function and dissociative symptoms) but do not allow causal conclusions: it remains unclear whether alterations are a predisposition for or the result of frequent dissociation and probably stem from an interplay of multiple factors. Longitudinal studies and/or studies applying approaches like dynamic causal modeling are needed to gain more insight into possible causal relationships. Moreover, other variables that were not assessed in these studies might have moderated (strengthened/weakened) or mediated (explained) the relationship. To further clarify this, additional longitudinal studies, addressing possible moderating or mediating factors (e.g., trauma), are needed. Further limitations include small sample sizes and sole inclusion of female samples. To gain clearer insight into the role of dissociation in brain structure and function, studies with larger data sets in both female and male patients and meta-analyses are needed to replicate/extend existing findings.

Possible Clinical Implications

Neuroimaging research on dissociation in BPD can have important clinical implications. Dissociative symptoms were found to hinder treatment outcome, possibly by interfering with habituation processes and new learning: such a negative effect of dissociative symptoms on treatment outcome has been shown for several disorders [46,47,48,49,50, 129•]. While dissociation can be adaptive in certain situations (e.g., in threatening situations that cannot be escaped or controlled), generalized and involuntary dissociative responses can have detrimental effects across multiple life domains [9]. Tailored interventions may help individuals to reduce/control dissociation at moments when such processes are disruptive and therefore maladaptive.

So far, neuroimaging studies have mainly focused on identifying the neural processes possibly underlying dissociation. As a next step, neuroimaging research may help to identify neural changes associated with a dissociative-symptom reduction after tailored interventions [148] and psychotherapy outcome [157,158,159].

Neuroimaging tools such as real-time fMRI may be used to translate knowledge on possible neural pathways involved in dissociation into experimental interventions. Real-time fMRI neurofeedback might be a promising add-on tool in combination with pharmaco- and psychotherapeutic treatment [148]. As a first step, more pilot studies are needed to identify which brain regions or functional processes/signals may be suitable targets for such interventions, as so far very little is known about the neural mechanisms of change that are key modulators in this relationship.