Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The affect heuristic and public support for three types of wood smoke mitigation policies

  • Published:
Air Quality, Atmosphere & Health Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study applied the affect heuristic model to investigate key psychological factors (affective associations, perceived benefits, and costs of wood heating) contributing to public support for three distinct types of wood smoke mitigation policies: education, incentives, and regulation. The sample comprised 265 residents of Armidale, an Australian regional community adversely affected by winter wood smoke pollution. Our results indicate that residents with stronger positive affective associations with wood heating expressed less support for wood smoke mitigation policies involving regulation. This relationship was fully mediated by expected benefits and costs associated with wood heating. Affective associations were unrelated to public support for policies involving education and incentives, which were broadly endorsed by all segments of the community, and were more strongly associated with rational considerations. Latent profile analysis revealed no evidence to support the proposition that some community members experience internal “heart versus head” conflicts in which their positive affective associations with wood heating would be at odds with their risk judgments about the dangers of wood smoke pollution. Affective associations and cost/benefit judgments were very consistent with each other.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The measurements were obtained from the Armidale Dumaresq Council.

  2. First author may be contacted to obtain a copy of the full questionnaire.

  3. Examination of the raw correlations in Table 1 indicated that affective associations were strongly correlated with support for regulation policy but uncorrelated with support for policies related to education and rebates. Thus, we assessed mediation for regulation policy only.

References

  • Akaike H (1974) A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Control 19:716–723

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alhakami AS, Slovic P (1994) A psychological study of the inverse relationship between perceived risk and perceived benefit. Risk Anal 14:1085–1096

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Armidale Dumaresq Council (2013) State of the environment report 2012–2013

  • Australian Bureau of Statistics (2011) Census of population and housing. Canberra ACT

  • Australian Bureau of Statistics (2012) Year Book Australia 2012: energy use and conservation. (Catalogue No. 1301.0)

  • Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: considerations, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Per Soc Psychol 51:1173–1182

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlop SA, Cotter T, Perez D (2014) When your smoking is not just about you: antismoking advertising, interpersonal pressure, and quitting outcomes. J Health Commun 19:41–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elder RW, Shults RA, Sleet DA, Nichols JL, Thompson RS, Rajab W (2004) Effectiveness of mass media campaigns for reducing drinking and driving and alcohol-involved crashes: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med 27:57–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S (1994) Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. Am Psychol 49:709–724

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Finucane ML, Alhakami ALI, Slovic P, Johnson SM (2000) The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. J Behav Decis Mak 17:1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finucane ML, Peters E, Slovic P (2003) Judgment and decision making: the dance of affect and reason. In: Schneider SL, Shanteau J (eds) Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision research. Cambridge University Press, UK, pp 327–364

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hine DW, Marks ADG, Nachreiner M, Gifford R, Heath Y (2007) Keeping the home fires burning: the affect heuristic and wood smoke pollution. J Environ Psychol 27:26–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hine DW, Gibson X, Marks AD, O’Neill G (2009) Smoking cessation in adults: a dual process perspective. Addict Res Theory 17:220–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hine DW, Bhullar N, Marks ADG, Kelly P, Scott JG (2011) Comparing the effectiveness of education and technology in reducing wood smoke pollution: a field experiment. J Environ Psychol 31:282–288

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson EJ, Tversky A (1983) Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. J Personal Soc Psychol 45:20–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leiserowitz A (2006) Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: the role of affect, imagery, and values. Climate Change 77:45–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner J, Gonzalez R, Small D, Fischhoff B (2003) Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: a national field experiment. Psychol Sci 14:144–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB (2001) Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika 88:767–778

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maibach EW, Leiserowitz A, Roser-Renouf C, Mertz CK (2011) Identifying like-minded audiences for global warming public engagement campaigns: an audience segmentation analysis and tool development. PLos ONE 6:e17571

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén LK, Muthén BO (2006) MPlus user’s guide, 4th edn. Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Naeher LP, Brauer M, Lipsett M, Zelikoff JT, Simpson CD, Koenig JQ, Smith KR (2007) Woodsmoke health effects: a review. Inhal Toxicol 19:67–106

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus DL (1998) The Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR-7). Multi-Health Systems, Toronton/Buffalo

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters E, Slovic P (1996) The role of affect and worldviews as orienting dispositions in the perception and acceptance of nuclear power. J App Soc Psychol 26:1427–1453

    Google Scholar 

  • Preacher KJ, Hayes AF (2008) Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods 40:879–891

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat 6:461–464

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siergrist M, Stampfli N, Kastenholz H, Keller C (2008) Perceived risks and perceive benefits of different nanotechnology foods and nanotechnology food packaging. Appetite 49:283–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic P (2001) Cigarette smokers: rational actors or rational fools? In: Slovic P (ed) Smoking: risk, perception & policy. Sage Publications, Inc., US, pp 97–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic P, Peters E (2006) Risk perception and affect. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 15:322–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic P, Finucane M, Peters E, Macgregor DG (2002) The affect heuristic. In: Griffin D, Kahenman D (eds) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 397–420

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich KE (2004) The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tay R (2005) Mass media campaigns reduce the incidence of drinking and driving. Evid Based Healthc Public Health 9:26–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor P, Wakefield M, Flay B, Nichter M (2003) Effects of anti-smoking advertising on youth smoking: a review. J Health Commun 8:229–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NSW Environment Climate Change and Water (2010) Regulatory Impact Statement: Proposed Protection of the Environment Operations (Clean Air) Regulation 2010. Sydney

  • Zajonc RB (1980) Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. Am J Individ Psychol 35:151–175

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Navjot Bhullar.

Appendix

Appendix

The 12 items were subjected to a principal axis factor analysis to identify the latent structure. The analysis produced three factors with eigenvalues greater than 1, explaining 68 % of the variance in the item set. The scree plot also indicated that three factors should be retained. Following a Direct-Oblimin rotation, all items had loadings greater than .60, and there were no cross-loadings above .20. The first factor, labeled “support for regulation,” included six items primarily dealing with placing restrictions on wood burning and implementing stricter standards for wood heaters. Three items relating to providing rebates for purchasing tools and technology to reduce emissions loaded on the second factor labeled “support for incentives.” The last factor, “support for education” consisted of two items related to providing information to the general public about proper firewood management and wood operation. Policy support scores were computed by taking the mean response for items on each subscale. Pattern matrix item loadings for each subscale are presented in the table below.

Table 3 Outcome variables: means, standard errors, and mean differences across profiles

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bhullar, N., Hine, D.W., Marks, A. et al. The affect heuristic and public support for three types of wood smoke mitigation policies. Air Qual Atmos Health 7, 347–356 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11869-014-0243-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11869-014-0243-1

Keywords

Navigation